Accessibility / Report Error

The controversy about a possible relationship between mobile phone use and cancer

A controvérsia sobre a possível relação entre o uso do telefone celular e câncer

Abstracts

Over the last decade, mobile phone use increased to almost 100% prevalence in many countries. Evidence for potential health hazards accumulated in parallel by epidemiologic investigations has raised controversies about the appropriate interpretation and the degree of bias and confounding responsible for reduced or increased risk estimates. Overall, 33 epidemiologic studies were identified in the peer-reviewed literature, mostly (25) about brain tumors. Methodologic considerations revealed that three important conditions for epidemiologic studies to detect an increased risk are not met:no evidence-based exposure metric is available; the observed duration of mobile phone use is generally still too low; no evidence-based selection of end points among the grossly different types of neoplasias is possible because of lack of etiologic hypotheses. The overall evidence speaks in favor of an increased risk, but its magnitude cannot be assessed at present because of insufficient information on long-term use.

Acoustic neuroma; Brain tumors; Glioma; Meningioma; Mobile phones; Risk assessment


Na última década, a prevalência do uso do telefone celular aumentou cerca de 100% em muitos países. Evidências de potenciais riscos acumulados para saúde em paralelo por pesquisas epidemiológicas levantam controvérsias sobre a interpretação apropriada e o grau de influência e desordem responsável pela redução ou aumento das estimativas de risco. Ao todo, 33 estudos epidemiológicos foram revistos por pares, sendo a maioria (25) sobre tumores cerebrais. Considerações metodológicas revelaram que não foram atendidas três importantes condições de estudos epidemiológicos para detectar um aumento de risco: não há nenhuma métrica de exposição baseada em evidências; a duração do uso de telefone celular observada é geralmente ainda muito baixa; não é possível a seleção de parâmetros com base em evidências entre os tipos de neoplasias totalmente diferentes, devido à falta de hipóteses etiológicas. As evidências no geral expressam um risco aumentado, mas sua magnitude não pode ser avaliada no momento graças à informação insuficiente sobre o uso a longo prazo.

Neuroma do acústico; Tumores cerebrais; Glioma; Meningioma; Telefones celulares; Avaliação de risco


REVIEW REVISÃO

The controversy about a possible relationship between mobile phone use and cancer

A controvérsia sobre a possível relação entre o uso do telefone celular e câncer

Michael Kundi

Institute of Environmental Health, Medical University of Vienna. Kinderspitalgasse 15, A-1095 Vienna, Austria. michael.kundi@meduniwien.ac.at

ABSTRACT

Over the last decade, mobile phone use increased to almost 100% prevalence in many countries. Evidence for potential health hazards accumulated in parallel by epidemiologic investigations has raised controversies about the appropriate interpretation and the degree of bias and confounding responsible for reduced or increased risk estimates. Overall, 33 epidemiologic studies were identified in the peer-reviewed literature, mostly (25) about brain tumors. Methodologic considerations revealed that three important conditions for epidemiologic studies to detect an increased risk are not met:no evidence-based exposure metric is available; the observed duration of mobile phone use is generally still too low; no evidence-based selection of end points among the grossly different types of neoplasias is possible because of lack of etiologic hypotheses. The overall evidence speaks in favor of an increased risk, but its magnitude cannot be assessed at present because of insufficient information on long-term use.

Key words: Acoustic neuroma, Brain tumors, Glioma, Meningioma, Mobile phones, Risk assessment

RESUMO

Na última década, a prevalência do uso do telefone celular aumentou cerca de 100% em muitos países. Evidências de potenciais riscos acumulados para saúde em paralelo por pesquisas epidemiológicas levantam controvérsias sobre a interpretação apropriada e o grau de influência e desordem responsável pela redução ou aumento das estimativas de risco. Ao todo, 33 estudos epidemiológicos foram revistos por pares, sendo a maioria (25) sobre tumores cerebrais. Considerações metodológicas revelaram que não foram atendidas três importantes condições de estudos epidemiológicos para detectar um aumento de risco: não há nenhuma métrica de exposição baseada em evidências; a duração do uso de telefone celular observada é geralmente ainda muito baixa; não é possível a seleção de parâmetros com base em evidências entre os tipos de neoplasias totalmente diferentes, devido à falta de hipóteses etiológicas. As evidências no geral expressam um risco aumentado, mas sua magnitude não pode ser avaliada no momento graças à informação insuficiente sobre o uso a longo prazo.

Palavras-chave: Neuroma do acústico, Tumores cerebrais, Glioma, Meningioma, Telefones celulares, Avaliação de risco

Because of the enormous increase in mobile phone use starting in the mid-1990s and reaching almost 100% prevalence in many countries worldwide by now, concerns have been raised that even small risks for developing chronic diseases such as cancer from mobile phone use may have substantial impact on public health. In fact, never before in history has any device of comparative prevalent use been associated with such high exposure to high-frequency electromagnetic fields (EMFs). (In addition, exposure to extremely low-frequency magnetic fields occurs from battery discharge.) Although from the perspective of the thermal effects paradigm, the rate of energy deposition in tissues of the mobile phone user is below levels considered harmful, there has been debate since the 1930s that tissue heating may not be the only relevant effect elicited by exposure to high-frequency EMFs; thus, there may be a relevant risk that has not been established yet because of the scarcity of exposure conditions that are comparable across a significant proportion of the population. For all the diverse high-frequency exposures occurring in environmental and occupational settings ranging from long-waves [a type of amplitude modulation (AM) broadcasting with carrier frequencies between 153 and 280 kHz] to radar waves, only a few long-term observa-tional studies have been published (for an over-view, see Ahlbom et al.1; Krewski et al.2; Kundi et al.3). Similarly scarce are long-term animal studies of lowlevel exposures in the pre-mobile phone era. Hence, at the time mobile phones were introduced, there were insufficient data to decide about health risks from low-level exposures, but the prevailing opinion that no relevant health effects occur at exposures below guideline levels led to the expectation that mobile phones are safe. However, the exponential growth of mobile phone use came as a surprise to the industry as well as to scientists involved in EMF risk assessment. Therefore, the existing gaps in knowledge should be addressed in both experimental as well as epidemiologic investigations focusing on exposures occurring in mobile telecommunications.

Since the mid-1990s, many epidemiologic studies of mobile phone use have been conducted worldwide, most of them focusing on tumors of the head region. Despite the growing database, the concerns have not been settled and a controversy still exists about possible adverse health effects. Although some may be inclined to attribute the ongoing debate to the enormous economic impact of modern telecommunication during the past decade, this debate also mirrors fundamental difficulties and a permissible range of interpretation under circumstances of insufficient knowledge. Despite this state of affairs, not all arguments that have been put forward in this controversy are valid. In the following sections I will concentrate on epidemiologic findings and their interpretation, although experimental work deserves similar critical appraisal.

I will first give a brief overview of the results of epidemiologic investigations of mobile phone use and tumors of the head region. I will then address methodologic problems associated with these studies and, by application of the pragmatic approach proposed earlier4, discuss whether epidemiologic evidence supports a causal interpretation of an association between mobile phone use and brain tumors.

Overview of epidemiologic studies

Table 1 presents an overview of results of epidemiologic studies on the association between brain tumors and mobile phone use. Other end points include salivary gland tumors5-7, uveal melanoma8, non-Hodgkin lymphoma9,10), facial nerve tumors11, and testicular cancer12, but for these diseases the database is insufficient to date.

Except for the early cohort study from the United States13 that was stopped by the court after 1 year of follow-up and the Danish retrospective cohort study14,15, all investigations have been case-control studies.

The "Overall results" from 25 epidemiologic studies listed in Table 1 do not demonstrate support for an increased risk. Only a few risk estimates are significantly elevated, and some are even significantly reduced. Risk estimates for longer duration of use are higher, on average, than overall estimates, and estimates for ipsilateral mobile phone use (i.e., use of the mobile phone on the same side where the tumor occurred), where available, tend to be even higher. Implications of these findings are discussed below.

Methodologic problems

Although a number of established study designs in epidemiology have been successfully applied in thousands of investigations in the past 50 years, perhaps few epidemiologists are fully aware of the conditions necessary to detect an existing risk by application of these methodologies. Analytical epidemiology intends to estimate the risk as a function of exposure to an agent by application of one of three classical study types: cross-sectional, case-control, and cohort study designs. Theoretically, all three types are capable of detecting an existing risk under ideal conditions but differ in their sensitivity to the effects of extraneous and confounding factors.

For all study designs, it has to be assumed that exposure to the agent can be assessed with a certain sensitivity and specificity. In the case of mobile phone use, the appropriate exposure metric is unknown. Absorption of electromagnetic energy in the body of the user depends on technical features of the phone and the network, as well as on anatomical features and habits of use. At first glance, this situation does not seem to be much different from, for example, an exposure to an air pollutant that also varies in time and space and which internal dose will depend on physiologic conditions. The problem with mobile telephones is much more profound. What aspect of the complex exposure condition described by "mobile phone use" could be responsible for an effect? It is obvious that given a certain indicator of mobile phone use, such as years of regular use, cumulative number of calls, cumulative hours of use, or cumulative absorption of electromagnetic energy in a certain area of the body, there are indefinitely many exposure conditions that are equivalent under the chosen metric and hence induce an equivalence relation in the space of exposure patterns that cannot be assessed as to its suitability with respect to any outcome measure without a sound mechanistic theory.

Another essential problem is related to the long induction periods and latencies of tumors in the head and neck region. Mobile phone use that was insignificant before the mid-1990s could not be studied with respect to its influence during induction period because, in almost all users, malignant transformation has likely occurred long before exposure to mobile phones commenced. Although an influence during the initiation phase cannot be ruled out, many researchers have expressed the opinion that if there is an effect at all, it is an effect on tumor promotion or progression8,17,19. If this is actually the case, several scenarios have to be considered. For example, in a summary of acoustic neuroma, Mohyuddin et al.41 found that tumor growth is exhibited in 48-70% of patients, stable tumor volumes in 27- 50%, and involution in 2-10%. If mobile phone use influences growth rate, this influence could result in either restarting growth in stable tumors, an increase of growth rate in growing tumors, or an inhibition of involution. The net result would be an earlier onset of symptoms and an earlier diagnosis. If there is an effect of the specified type, the age incidence function in the exposed population would be shifted compared with that of the unexposed population by a fraction of the exposure duration, as long as duration of use is short compared with the natural history of the disease. Given that the age incidence function has a positive slope, this shift is equivalent to an increased incidence in the exposed population for any given age. For simplicity, assume that the duration and effect of exposure is constant across all age groups. Taking the slope for the age incidence function for brain tumors as 0.04 [on the log incidence scale42], the estimated incidence of brain cancer at any given age would be proportional to exp(age × 0.04) in the population as a whole. If the exposed segment of the population has an age incidence function shifted by 2 years, the estimated incidence at any given age among the exposed would be proportional to exp[(age + 2) × 0.04], resulting in an OR of exp(2 × 0.04) = 1.08 for mobile phone use. Note that the result is independent of the exposure prevalence and depends only on the shift of the age incidence function and its slope. Given the lower number of mobile phone users at an older age, the expected ORs from studies with short exposure durations (and hence small shifts in the age incidence function) are too small to be detected with acceptable power.

The third fundamental problem is related to the vast diversity of tumor types to be considered. The World Health Organization differentiates about 50 types of brain tumors43; there are more than a dozen different histologic salivary gland tumors, and so forth. Furthermore, in recent years molecular histopathology revealed many differences within certain types of tumors. For example, there are at least two clusters of glioblastoma multiforme, the most frequent malignant brain tumor in adults: one expresses loss of heterozygosity on chromosome 17p with mutation of the p53 tumor suppressor gene, and the other cluster is characterized by an amplification of the epidermal growth factor receptor oncogene44. Is it possible that all of these diverse types of tumors respond uniformly to mobile phone radiation? We cannot hope to extract sensitive types by epidemiologic investigations because of the small numbers for each distinct type.

In summary, the three most important conditions for epidemiology to arrive at a conclusion concerning a potential risk of an agent are as follows:

. It must be possible to measure (at least by a surrogate marker) the component of the agent that is related to the risk.

. For agents that promote the disease in question, duration of exposure must be a substantial fraction of the history of the disease.

. There must be good a priori reason to select specific types of diseases that are sufficiently homogenous to support the assumption of more or less uniform etiology.

None of these preconditions are met in the study of mobile phone use and cancer. As a consequence, even substantially increased risks might go undetected, and evidence will tend to be equivocal.

Do mobile phones cause brain tumors?

To assess a possible causal relationship between an agent and cancer, a pragmatic dialog approach has been delineated4. According to this procedure, epidemiologic evidence must be assessed concerning four aspects: temporal relation, association, environmental, and population equivalence. If there are no valid counter arguments against the evidence for an association, this suffices for a verdict of causation. If epidemiologic evidence is insufficient, other evidence that increases or decreases confidence in a causal relationship could be included to come to a conclusion.

Temporal relation

Assessment of temporal relation is not a trivial problem. It is impossible to define the point in time when a brain tumor started. Before a tumor can be diagnosed, which in the case of brain tumors occurs either by coincidence, if it is detected by imaging techniques applied for other reasons, or because of symptoms produced by the growing cell mass, the tumor was present for many years or even decades. For meningioma, average induction periods of about 20-40 years have been calculated in adults, based on observations of patients exposed to ionizing radiation45. For acoustic neuroma, slow growth, with an average volume doubling time of about 1.7 years, suggests similar induction periods41. For glioma, case reports46 and long-term follow-up after childhood radiation therapy of tinea capitis47 also suggest induction periods of decades. Considering a temporal relationship between exposure and the diverse steps of brain tumor development, the following four phases may be differentiated: 1) exposure commenced before the first step of malignant transformation; 2) exposure started during the induction phase, which could itself last for several years; 3) onset of exposure occurred during the noninvasive growth phase; and 4) exposure started during final (autonomous) growth. In cases 1 and 2, exposure might influence malignant transformation itself and cause de novooccurrence of a brain tumor. In case 3, exposure might influence the fate of the deviating clone and could decrease latency or probability of spontaneous involution and therefore either increase incidence because of a shift of latency or because a tumor that would otherwise remain obscure during lifetime manifests itself clinically. In case 4, no contribution of exposure is possible. Unfortunately, little is known about the duration of these phases. Furthermore, there are likely gross differences between tumor types concerning absolute and relative length of these steps during natural history of the disease. Although slowly growing tumors such as most meningioma and schwannoma may have unchanged growth rates during prolonged periods of time, other brain tumors such as glioblastoma show an explosive final growth after possibly long periods of more stable behavior. For radiation-induced tumors, Cahan et al.48 proposed to allow at least 5 years for induction periods. As delineated above, for an effect during latent growth to be detected in epidemiologic studies, exposure must have been during a substantial proportion of growth phase. Therefore, for an influence both during the induction phase and on tumor growth rate, at least 5 years must be allowed for latency or duration of exposure, respectively, to fulfill the criterion of temporal relation. Because for virtually all carcinogens, repetitive or prolonged exposures are necessary to bring about an increased cancer incidence, it is necessary to consider not only time since first exposure but also duration of exposure. Number of calls and average duration of calls seem to be too difficult to remember for periods far in the past, but information about periods of regular use is more easily recalled and therefore could be the best choice for exposure determination. (In principle, it may even be validated by network provider data).Years of regular mobile or cordless phone use up to 5 years before diagnosis would possibly be the appropriate exposure meter for most slowly growing tumors. Because such evaluations have not been performed, I instead assessed exposure duration or latency of >10 years, as available, for Table 1. In these subjects, at least half of the exposure duration falls within an etiologically relevant period.

Association

I computed a meta-analytical estimate of the risk for the different brain tumor types based on all independent studies reporting ORs for > 10 years of mobile phone use. Heterogeneity was assessed by chi-square tests. A p-value < 0.05 was considered significant. If studies included the same population, only those without overlap were considered in the meta-analyses. Pooled effects and SEs were derived from the fixed-effect model because no significant heterogeneity was detected. Because basically all investigations used the same study design and the same end point definition, despite some deviations in exposure classification and proneness to misclassification and selection bias, no attempt was made in the meta-analysis to account for these differences. It should, however, be noted that correction for selection and misclassification bias would lead to higher meta-analytical odds ratios. There is no publication bias in this case because all studies that are planned, on-going, or completed are known to the scientific community.

For glioma, I included three studies33,36,49 reporting data on 233 exposed cases and 330 exposed controls among 2,792 glioma patients and 6,195 control subjects. I found no heterogeneity across studies, and the combined OR was 1.5 [95% confidence interval (CI), 1.2-1.8]. For acoustic neuroma, two independent pooled analyses31,50 gave an overall OR of 1.3 (95% CI, 0.95- 1.9), based on 67 exposed cases and 311 exposed controls among 912 and 5,715 cases of acoustic neuroma and controls, respectively. Risk for meningioma from mobile phone use of > 10 years was reported in two individual studies33,50 and one pooled study40, with an overall OR of 1.1 (95% CI, 0.8-1.4) evaluated from 116 exposed cases and 320 controls among a total of 2,506 meningioma patients and 6,223 control subjects. Hence, there is an increased risk for all of these end points from mobile phone use that is statistically significant for glioma. In these analyses mobile phone use was assumed to induce the neoplasia. If mobile phone use has an additional or exclusive effect on tumor growth, this analysis is not entirely appropriate because an effect on the growing tumor can only be exerted by exposure on the same side of the head where the tumor is located. Combined estimates for ipsilateral mobile phone use of > 10 years give the ORs: 1.9 (95% CI, 1.4-2.4) for glioma; 1.6 (95% CI, 1.1-2.5) for acoustic neuroma; and 1.3 (95% CI, 0.9-1.9) for meningioma. Hence, there are clear indications of increased risks for all three end points.

According to the dialogue approach, association must be assessed whether or not there are valid counterarguments, and especially those based on considerations about the impact of possible biases.

Potential biases

As apparent from Table 1, ever (regular) use of a mobile phone rarely revealed increased risks for any type of brain tumor. Except for the Finnish study of Auvinen et al.21, only the Swedish group of Hardell and colleagues (e.g., Hardell et al.23) reported significantly elevated estimates of relative risks.

Most studies summarized in Table 1 were conducted based on the Interphone protocol51 that defined regular use as at least one outgoing or incoming call per week for at least 6 months, with ever-regular use starting 1 year before the reference date. Although the reference date was defined as date of diagnosis in cases and the same date of the matched control, in studies not individually matched (e.g., Hepworth et al.32), there are problems in defining the reference date because of the interview lag time. Because of the rapid increase in mobile phone use during and before the study period, the methods applied to compute the reference date for controls could be a source of bias. Information provided in the study reports is insufficient to decide whether adjustments were biased. In some articles31,32, controls were allocated into categories of interview lag time at random without consideration of age and sex of the cases within these categories, whereas in others26, average lag between diagnosis and identification in a matched set was subtracted from date of control identification. The first method introduces bias if distribution of age and sex within categories of lag times differs, and the second method introduces bias if the date of identification differs between cases and controls. To my knowledge, these possible biases have not been considered previously. In the Interphone study51, data were collected from the end of 2000 through the beginning of 2004, with some differences between countries. During this period, mobile phone penetration rate increased from about 60% to about 90% in the European Union, according to the International Telecommunication Union. Insufficient adjustment for differences in the interview date would result in underestimation of risk.

In their studies, Hardell and colleagues disregarded mobile and cordless phone use within the last year before the reference date. Any use of a mobile or cordless phone was counted except when hands-free devices or external car antennas were used. The reference date for controls was set to the date of diagnosis of the matched case. In some reports of pooled data sets (e.g., Hardell et al.49), individual matching was disregarded and controls from different studies were included. Insufficient adjustment of the reference date could have also led to bias in this case.

Hardell and colleagues defined the unex-posed subjects as those who have not used a mobile or cordless phone for > 1 year before diagnosis (or reference date in controls). The Interphone group disregarded cordless phones in analyses of mobile phone use (and vice versa). Cardis et al.51 and Takebayashi et al.35 have argued that cordless phone use is associated with much lower exposure to microwaves and therefore cannot be counted in exposure assessments. This view is not correct. Average power levels are not much different between cordless phones (average levels of 10 mW) and mobile phones (median average output power 6-16 mW in urban areas)52. Considering the typically longer duration of daily use of cordless phones compared with mobile phones, it is not a rational procedure to exclude them from total exposure (for information on adolescent users, see Söderqvist et al.53. The fraction of cordless phone users among cases and controls not using mobile phones ranged from about 22% in the study of Hardell et al.50 to almost 40% in the German Interphone study33. If we arbitrarily assign sensitivity of exposure determination from omission of cordless phone use a value of 74% in cases and 78% in controls, assuming 100% specificity (in cases and controls) and an actual exposure prevalence of 54% [according to the data of Hardell et al.50], a true OR of 1.5 would be reduced to 1.2. Still greater reductions of the OR result if the differences in cordless phone use were actually greater.

In the Interphone studies, data acquisition concerning exposure was predominantly done by computer-assisted personal interview (CAPI). In about 95% of glioma cases and controls, exposure assessment was based on CAPI51. Reports from five Nordic countries36 reveal that > 40% of cases were interviewed in the hospital. However, this fraction ranges from almost 100% in Finland to 3% in the United Kingdom. Data acquisition was completely different in the studies of Hardell and colleagues23. They sent a questionnaire to home addresses of cases and controls, and upon return, they evaluated the questionnaires for errors, omissions, and discrepancies. If necessary, additional information was sought by telephone interviews blinded to case status.

Method of data acquisition could be important in several respects: a) interviews not blinded to case status may introduce a bias from the expectations of the interviewer; b) the interaction between interviewee and interviewer as such can lead to bias (Rosenthal effects); c) answering a questionnaire at home is less demanding (especially considering the conditions after surgery) than personal interviews; d) at home it is possible to check telephone bills or to inspect contracts with network providers to verify data. For these reasons, the questionnaire method seems to be superior to the interview technique. However, there are also advantages of the CAPI method: data can be immediately checked for errors and discrepancies, and the interviewer can explain points that are not clear and may help in recalling inquired items. Validation studies54-56 within the Interphone study showed only a moderate correlation between self-reported intensity of mobile phone use and traffic data from network operators, but confirmed usage data as valid proxy for microwave exposure. For the (not very important) recent mobile phone use, self reports seem to be fairly accurate, but for earlier use, no data on reliability are available. Considering results from Christensen et al.27, memory performance is decreased especially in patients with high-grade glioma. Exposure assessment in these patients could be particularly biased if conducted by interviews compared with the questionnaire method. Bias often represents underreporting of mobile phone use, because it is more likely that a patient forgot using a mobile phone once years ago compared with falsely stating mobile phone use.

In the Interphone studies, overall participation was 65% for glioma cases, 78% for meningioma, and 82% for acoustic neuroma51. For controls, participation was 53% but there was large variation across centers, ranging from 35% to 74%. Hardell et al.57, using postal questionnaires, reported participation rates of 88-91% in cases and 84-92% in controls. The participation rate in cases was computed based on eligible cases that received a questionnaire, defined as those with ascertained primary brain tumors alive at the time of identification and whose participation was not denied by their physician. If the definition of eligible cases for the Interphone studies were applied, the participation rate would amount to about 65- 85% in the different studies of Hardell and colleagues. In the Interphone studies, on average, 13% (range, 2-44%) of case interviews were performed as proxy interviews. As shown by Vrijheid et al.55 in an Interphone validation study, response bias due to differential selection of groups of the population with higher prevalence of mobile phone use possibly has the highest impact, even outweighing recall bias. Lönn et al.30 showed that non-participating cases had almost the same proportion of mobile phone users (50% compared with 52% in participants), but non-participating controls differed markedly from participants (34% compared with 59%). Effect of this selection bias might be even greater if long-term use is considered. Consequence of selection bias can easily be determined because the biased OR is equal to the product of the true OR and the selection OR (cf. Rothman et al.58 ). Given the non-response analysis of Lönn et al.30 ), the selection OR is computed as 0.64 if overall participation rates of the Interphone studies are considered (72% and 53% in cases and controls, respectively). All 46 ORs in the report of Lönn et al.30 , except one in the overall analysis of glioma and meningioma, were < 1. Assuming the selection OR is 0.75 as computed based on the participation rates for the Lönn study specifically (79% in cases and 70.5% in controls), almost all these ORs would increase above 1 and none would be significantly < 1 (as was the case for 7 of the 46 ORs). For example, the OR for > 10 years of mobile phone use for glioma, reported as 0.9, would increase to 1.2.

As has been pointed out previously3,31,59, early symptoms of a developing brain tumor may have influenced behavior regarding mobile phone use. In particular, growing acoustic neuroma are frequently associated with hearing problems and tinnitus. Such symptoms may lead to a restriction of use, change of the side of the head the phone is held during calls, and even to discontinuing mobile phone use. Lönn et al.26, in their study of acoustic neuroma, assessed impact of hearing loss and tinnitus 5 years before reference date and reported no differences in risk estimates for patients with and without hearing loss. Whether this is an indication that such symptoms have no impact on mobile phone use and therefore do not bias risk estimates is difficult to assess because no data were reported. As noted by Schoemaker et al.31, 59% of regular phone users among controls reported predominantly right-sided use, 33% left-sided use, and 8% use on both sides. The authors argued that if mobile phones cause acoustic neuromas, one might expect a higher proportion of tumors on the right than on the left side of the head among regular phone users. However, this expectation is completely unfounded. In contrast, tumor growth may cause behavioral changes, as indicated by the distribution of mobile phone use in cases, with only 49% right-sided users, 40% left-sided users, and 11% that used the phone on both sides.

Although response bias, misclassification bias, and insufficient correction of interview lag time between cases and controls will reduce risk estimates toward or even below unity, some biases could lead to a spuriously increased risk. One particular point has been raised frequently: Increased risk estimates of ipsilateral mobile phone use (Table 1) could be due to recall bias. If mobile phone use affects tumor development and growth, it is important to consider the side of the head to which the phone is held during calls. Cardis et al.60 reported that 97-99% of the total electromagnetic energy deposited in the brain is absorbed at the side of the head the phone is held during calls. Because of this asymmetry, an effect at the site of the growing tumor is expected only or primarily for ipsilateral use. There is no objective method to retrospectively assess side of the head the phone has been used. Asking a person about this aspect of use could result in bias. A person may be inclined to suspect mobile phone use as a causal factor and may therefore tend to report using it at the same side as the tumor has occurred. On the other hand, the reverse also may be claimed-that a person wants to dismiss the possibility that using the phone has anything to do with the disease and is therefore falsely reporting the opposite side of use. Even if a patient does not intentionally distort the answer, recent surgery may cause memory deficiencies leading to recall bias. Hepworth et al.32 argued that the reduced risk on the contralateral side indicates such recall bias. However, this risk reduction was due to an artifact of the method applied. Estimate of relative risk for contralateral mobile phone use was based on non-regular and ipsilateral phone users as reference. It follows that whenever the relative risk of ipsilateral phone use is > 1, the relative risk of contralateral use must be < 1 [the expected value of the OR in this case is (πo+πi)/(πo+ψπi), where πo is the proportion in the population of nonusers, πI is the proportion in the population of ipsilateral users, and ψ is the OR for ipsilateral use]. All meta-analytical ORs for ipsilateral mobile phone use are > 1, and those for glioma and acoustic neuroma are statistically significant. If there was no misclassification bias in controls and perfect sensitivity, then a small recall bias in the direction of a preference for reporting mobile phone use at the side of the tumor of about 3% would reduce these enhanced ORs for long-term (> 10 years) use to 1. However, considering overall results for ipsilateral use, recall bias must reduce specificity in cases by 30- 40% to remove the observed enhanced risk. The specificity that reduces an observed increased OR to 1 is given by [1+(ψ*-1)π]-1, where ψ* is the observed OR and π is the exposure prevalence in the population, given that sensitivity in both cases and controls and specificity in controls are 158 . For example, taking the study of Hardell et al.29 with an overall OR for ipsilateral mobile and cordless phone use of approximately 3.0 for acoustic neuroma and a prevalence of about 23% of ipsilateral mobile or cordless phone use, the specificity must be as low as 68% to remove the observed effect. That is, 32% of those not exposed at all or not on the side of the tumor must have falsely stated they have been exposed. In other words, more than half of mobile phone users among cases and none among controls must have given the wrong side of the head for their predominant use to remove the observed increased risk. It should also be noted that the case-only approach of Takebayashi et al.39 cannot solve the problem of recall bias.

As can be seen in Table 1, several Interphone groups27,30,34,36,40 reported ORs that were significantly < 1, implying a protective effect of mobile phone use for brain tumors. Although there is a remote possibility that mobile phone use may enhance apoptosis or activate DNA repair, such processes will hardly affect tumor development at an advanced stage; thus, this is not a valid explanation for these reduced risk estimates but rather it suggests systematic bias. Selection bias, as delineated above, could explain some of these results. However, there are likely additional biases that contributed to the overall effect. Let us consider this aspect from the perspective of the ceteris paribuscondition (i.e., that cases and controls have essentially the same features in all relevant aspects except exposure) and in particular from the condition of population equivalence (i.e., the condition that both cases and controls stem from populations that are equivalent for all attributes that are relevant for the disease under study). Mobile phone use is not randomly distributed within the population, but usage patterns will be associated with certain attributes such as occupation, sex, socioeconomic status (SES), and age. Some of these attributes can be accounted for by matching or during analysis, but there could be an association with the disease that cannot be removed by these procedures. Exploration of prior symptoms in brain tumor cases often reveals indications of the disease process many years in the past (e.g., epileptic seizures, personality changes, a variety of cognitive and perception problems). Some of these symptoms could reduce the probability that a person chooses to use a mobile phone-or a telephone in general. Such habit changes would have the greatest impact on measures of cumulative duration and intensity of use. If aspects of the disease influence mobile phone use, the ceteris paribuscondition is violated from the very beginning. It is evident that this also implies a violation of the condition of temporal relation, because a reversal of cause and effect may occur. These difficulties are related to the generally short duration of mobile phone use. A solution could be inclusion of case history information and formation of distinct subgroups differing in duration of symptoms related to the developing disease. Obviously, influence of symptoms on mobile phone use will predominantly reduce risk estimates, because the odds for mobile phone use in cases will be lowered.

Because of the mentioned biases that could operate in case-control studies, one may be inclined to put some weight on the only cohort study19,20 presently available. However, this investigation is severely flawed and cannot contribute to risk assessment and was therefore not included in the meta-analysis (see also Hardell et al.61).

Confounding

Environmental equivalence seems to be sufficient in all investigations and confounding seems an unlikely cause of bias because there are only a few known risk factors for brain tumors that would induce bias if disregarded in the analyses. Therapeutic and, to a lesser degree, diagnostic X rays to the head region increase the risk for several types of brain tumors. In some studies this was considered a possible confounder but without effect on the risk estimates (e.g., Hardell et al.16), indicating that there is no correlation between such irradiation and mobile phone use. Other possible confounders include neurofibromatosis and tuberous sclerosis, family history of brain tumors, and medical treatment with growth factors, all of which are very rare conditions without reason to assume a relationship with mobile phone use. Age and sex are the most important confounders that have been considered in all studies either by matching or during analysis. SES has also been indicated as a possible confounder and was included in most analyses. However, including SES in the analysis will not remove selection bias that seems to be related to SES in some Interphone investigations (e.g., Hepworth et al.32).

Assessment

Overall, arguments in favor of or against an association between mobile phone use and brain tumors are not equally strong. There is evidence for selection bias, exposure misclassification from excluding cordless phone use, reversal of cause and effect from neglecting early symptoms of the disease, and short exposure duration. All these factors lead to reduction of the observed risk estimates. The only strong argument against an association is the possible impact of recall bias. There may be underreporting especially of early mobile phone use because of memory deficits after surgery, but most concerns have been raised for a potential distortion in reporting side of the head the phone is held during calls. Handedness correlates not very highly with side of the head the phone is used [concordances of only about 60% have been determined32], and there are no other methods at hand to validate these data. There are, however, some arguments that speak against a decisive influence of recall bias: Most participants of the Interphone and Hardell studies were enrolled during 1997-2003 at a time when mobile phone use was not widely discussed as a potential risk factor for brain tumors. As reported by Hardell et al.23, among 232 brain tumor cases who expressed their views about potential causes of their dis-ease, only two named mobile phones. Lönn62 asked 70 brain tumor cases and controls whether they considered mobile phones as a risk factor for brain tumors and found no difference between both groups. Even if patients consider mobile phones a factor contributing to their disease, what would they gain if they gave the wrong side of use? Most people choose to use mobile phones, but do they want to blame themselves for their disease? In several studies, mobile phone use was only one of the different risk factors investigated (e.g., Lönn et al.6). Bias from patients' attribution would likely extend also to these other factors, and spuriously increased risks should have been observed in some of them. Although reporting bias of this type may explain an increased risk for long-term ipsilateral use, it is hardly an explanation for the increased overall risk of ipsilateral use revealed in several investigations23,28,29,32.

When considering the discussion above, the conditions for a causal interpretation of the observed association between mobile phone use and brain tumors are not fully met. However, discussion of the potential violation of the conditions of temporal relation as well as population and environmental equivalence revealed that it must in all likelihood have reduced the observed association. Because of the important result on ipsilateral mobile phone use, a spuriously increased risk due to recall bias cannot be completely dismissed. In such a situation, evidence from other sources may increase or decrease confidence in a causal relation between mobile phone use and cancer.

Additional evidence

Since the 1930s there has been a scientific controversy about effects of high-frequency EMF other than thermal. Absorption of electromagnetic energy is now well understood and poses no principal difficulty of integration into Maxwell's theory of electromagnetism. The rate of absorption of electromagnetic energy in a homogenous volume of biological tissue is proportional to the temperature increase within this volume. Therefore, at high levels of EMF exposure, significant heating occurs that can be dangerous to health. Exposure standards have been issued that limit exposure to thermally safe levels. However, telecommunication and broadcasting applications of high-frequency EMF are not only radiating energy but also information. This is done by modulation of the highfrequency carrier. The modulation frequencies could be of biological significance and evoke nonthermal effects. In addition, the high-frequency signal itself could cause effects at low levels in combination with the simultaneous presence of the earth magnetic field63. In principle, interactions between EMF and matter are subject to two descriptions, classical or quantum electrodynamics. In a living cell, many important processes occur by electron transfer across membrane structures in a well-organized manner, ions cross selective channels, proteins get activated and deactivated by cascades of precisely regulated enzymes-all processes that occur on a quantum scale. Recently, by application of "omics" research (for an overview, see Vanderstraeten and Verschaeve64), it has been shown that cells may respond with activation of proteins and genes at nonthermal levels of exposure, a process that was also observed in vivo65. However, because of the lack of a mechanistic model, these results are uncertain as to their interpretation with respect to relevant long-term health effects. It has long been speculated66,67 that free radical formation is involved in EMF-induced health effects. Although there is some evidence of formation of free radicals at nonthermal levels of EMF68, there are many difficulties with this approach. It would be much too simplistic to assume that free radicals are directly produced by the interaction with the EMF. Rather, these radicals are produced by the cell itself as an intermediate step of the response to sensing the field69.

Concerning prior epidemiologic evidence of a relationship between high-frequency EMF other than those used in mobile telecommunication and brain tumors, despite some reports of increased risk70-73, the evidence is inconclusive to date.

Results of epidemiologic studies of mobile phone use summarized above indicate an association that is of moderate strength and in the range delineated for passive smoking and lung cancer. There is no meaningful indicator of exposure dose available, but longer latencies are associated with higher risk estimates, and there are indications that risk is higher in rural areas where phones typically radiate at higher intensities74. These aspects do increase confidence in a causal relationship.

In the case that epidemiology faces problems due to short exposure durations, lifetime animal bioassays gain importance in establishing a carcinogenic risk. Unfortunately, standard procedures cannot be applied for exposure to microwaves from mobile phones. In hundreds of animal carcinogenicity assays, it has been shown that even for DNA reactive agents, exposure during most of the life span of the animals at the maximum tolerated dose is necessary to significantly increase incidence. Such high exposures are impossible for high-frequency EMF because of interference with heating. Hence, it is necessary to apply levels that are much too low for an increased incidence to be expected. Some solutions to this problem have been proposed: a) coexposure or prior exposure to a known carcinogen; b) implantation of tumor cells; and c) use of animal strains with a habitually increased tumor incidence. Because of the unknown mechanism of action, none of these attempts can be evaluated according to their suitability. More than 30 long-term and mediumterm animal assays have been published in the past decade, most of which do not comply with basic criteria75, and there is no suitable model for brain tumors. Application of ethylnitrosourea during gestation results in an increased incidence of brain tumors but increases incidence of many other tumors as well. A further problem is the much smaller size of laboratory rodents, resulting in a completely different exposure pattern at telecommunication frequencies. In contrast to humans using a mobile phone with a localized exposure at the side of the head the phone is held, animal exposure is a whole-body exposure. Although devices have been constructed that result in a predominant head irradiation76, pattern and distribution within the brain will still be completely different from human exposures. It is not surprising then that up to now only a few animal experiments have found some indication of an increased cancer risk77-79.

Interpretation of epidemiologic findings would be much easier if genotoxicity of mobile telecommunication signals could firmly be established. In vitro experiments have brought about diverse results that at present provide only equivocal evidence for genotoxic effects. Additionally, in the case of genotoxicity assays, procedures widely used for assessing environmental and nutritional toxicants may not be ideally suited for the study of EMF. For example, if, as suggested by Lai and Singh80, exposure-induced effects imply activation of the Fenton reaction, cells rich in free iron would be responsive while others would not. Hence, overall evidence without considering mechanistic hypotheses is of limited value. Nevertheless, about one quarter of published genotoxicity studies found an effect of low-level exposures81-82.

Overall, animal and in vitro experiments do not reduce confidence in a causal relationship but do not provide unequivocal support either. The main problem is the lack of a coherent research strategy that unifies strengths of different disciplines to unravel the intriguing problem of lowlevel EMF health effects from a biophysical perspective.

Conclusions

Epidemiologic evidence compiled in the past 10 years starts to indicate an increased risk, in particular for brain tumors (glioma, meningioma, acoustic neuroma), from mobile phone use. Considering biases that may have been operating in most studies, the risk estimates are rather too low, although recall bias could have increased risk estimates. The net result, when considering the different errors and their impact, is still rather an elevated risk. The magnitude of the brain tumor risk is moderate, but it has to be borne in mind that estimates are still from short durations of exposure. From the perspective of public health, an increase of brain tumor incidence of > 50% poses substantial problems for neurosurgical care, but the individual perspective is less dramatic: in industrial countries, the lifetime brain tumor risk is 4-8 per 1,000. If mobile phone use should increase these figures to 6-12 per 1,000, the individual risk is still low.

At present, evidence for a causal relation-ship between mobile phone use and brain tumors relies predominantly on epidemiology, in particular on the large studies of Hardell and colleagues, but there are no valid counter arguments and no strong evidence decreasing confidence in a causal relationship. Weak evidence in favor of a causal relationship is provided by some animal and in vitro studies, but overall, genotoxicity assays, both in vivo and in vitro, are inconclusive to date.

  • 1. Ahlbom A, Green A, Kheifets L, Savitz D, Swerdlow A. Epidemiology of health effects of radiofrequency exposure. Environ Health Perspect 2004; 112:1741-1754.
  • 2. Krewski D, Byus C, Glickman B, Lotz W, Mandeville R, McBride M, Prato FS, Weaver DF. Recent advances in research on radio-frequency fields and health. J Toxicol Environ Health B Crit Rev 2001; 4(1):145-159.
  • 3. Kundi M. Mobile phone use and cancer. Occup Environ Med 2004; 61(6):560-570, 487.
  • 4. Kundi M. Causality and the interpretation of epidemiologic evidence. Environ Health Perspect 2006; 114:969-974.
  • 5. Hardell L, Hallquist A, Hansson Mild K, Carlberg M, Gertzén H, Schildt E, Dahlqvist A. No association between the use of cellular or cordless telephones and salivary gland tumours. Occup Environ Med2004; 61(8):675-679.
  • 6. Lönn S, Ahlbom A, Christensen H, Johansen C, Schüz J, Edström S, Henriksson G, Lundgren J, Wennerberg J, Feychting M. Mobile phone use and risk of parotid gland tumor. Am J Epidemiol 2006; 164(7):637-643.
  • 7. Sadetzki S, Chetrit A, Jarus-Hakak A, Cardis E, Deutch Y, Duvdevani S, Zultan A, Novikov I, Freedman L, Wolf M. Cellular phone use and risk of benign and malignant parotid gland tumors-a nationwide case-control study. Am J Epidemiol 2008; 167(4):457-467.
  • 8. Stang A, Anastassiou G, Ahrens W, Bromen K, Bornfeld N, Jöckel K. The possible role of radiofrequency radiation in the development of uveal melanoma. Epidemiology 2001; 12(1):7-12.
  • 9. Hardell L, Eriksson M, Carlberg M, Sundström C, Hansson Mild K. Use of cellular or cordless telephones and the risk for non-Hodgkin's lymphoma. Int Arch Occup Environ Health 2005; 78(8):625-632.
  • 10. Linet M, Taggart T, Severson R, Cerhan J, Cozen W, Hartge P, Colt J. Cellular telephones and non-Hodgkin lymphoma. Int J Cancer 2006; 119(10):2382- 2388.
  • 11. Warren H, Prevatt A, Daly K, Antonelli P. Cellular tele-phone use and risk of intratemporal facial nerve tumor. Laryngoscope 2003; 113(4):663-667.
  • 12. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Ohlson C, Westberg H, Eriksson M, Hansson Mild K. Use of cellular and cordless tele-phones and risk of testicular cancer. Int J Androl 2007; 30(2):115-122.
  • 13. Dreyer N, Loughlin J, Rothman K. Cause-specific mortality in cellular telephone users. JAMA 1999; 282(19):1814-1816.
  • 14. Hardell L, Näsman A, Påhlson A, Hallquist A, Hansson Mild K. Use of cellular telephones and the risk for brain tumours: a case-control study. Int J Oncol1999; 15(1):113-116.
  • 15. Hardell L, Nasman A, Påhlson A, Hallquist A. Casecontrol study on radiology work, medical X-ray investigations, and use of cellular telephones as risk factors for brain tumors. MedGenMed 2000; 2(2):E2.
  • 16. Hardell L, Hansson Mild K, Påhlson A, Hallquist A. Ionizing radiation, cellular telephones and the risk for brain tumours. Eur J Cancer Prev 2001; 10(6):523-529.
  • 17. Muscat J, Malkin M, Thompson S, Shore R, Stellman S, McRee D, Neugut AI, Wynder EL. Handheld cellular telephone use and risk of brain cancer. JAMA 2000; 284(23):3001-3007.
  • 18. Inskip P, Tarone R, Hatch E, Wilcosky T, Shapiro W, Selker R, Fine HA, Black PM, Loeffler JS, Linet MS. Cellular-telephone use and brain tumors. N Engl J Med 2001; 344(2):79-86.
  • 19. Johansen C, Boice J Jr, McLaughlin J, Olsen J. Cellular telephones and cancer-a nationwide cohort study in Denmark. J Natl Cancer Inst 2001; 93(3):203-207.
  • 20. Schüz J, Jacobsen R, Olsen J, Boice J Jr, McLaughlin J, Johansen C. Cellular telephone use and cancer risk: update of a nationwide Danish cohort. J Natl Cancer Inst 2006; 98(23):1707-1713.
  • 21. Auvinen A, Hietanen M, Luukkonen R, Koskela R. Brain tumors and salivary gland cancers among cellular tele-phone users. Epidemiology 2002; 13(3):356-359.
  • 22. Muscat J, Malkin M, Shore R, Thompson S, Neugut A, Stellman S, Bruce J. Handheld cellular telephones and risk of acoustic neuroma. Neurology2002; 58(8):1304-1306.
  • 23. Hardell L, Hallquist A, Hansson Mild K, Carlberg M, Påhlson A, Lilja A. Cellular and cordless telephones and the risk for brain tumours. Eur J Cancer Prev 2002; 11(4):377-386.
  • 24. Hardell L, Hansson Mild K, Carlberg M. Case-control study on the use of cellular and cordless phones and the risk for malignant brain tumours. Int J Radiat Biol 2002; 78(10):931-936.
  • 25. Christensen H, Schüz J, Kosteljanetz M, Poulsen H, Thomsen J, Johansen C. Cellular telephone use and risk of acoustic neuroma. Am J Epidemiol 2004; 159(3):277-283.
  • 26. Lönn S, Ahlbom A, Hall P, Feychting M. Mobile phone use and the risk of acoustic neuroma. Epidemiology 2004; 15(6):653-659.
  • 27. Christensen H, Schüz J, Kosteljanetz M, Poulsen H, Boice J Jr, McLaughlin J, Johansen C. Cellular telephones and risk for brain tumors: a populationbased, incident case-control study. Neurology 2005; 64(7):1189-1195.
  • 28. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. Case-control study of the association between the use of cellular and cordless telephones and malignant brain tumors diag-nosed during 2000-2003. Environ Res 2006; 100(2):232-241.
  • 29. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. Case-control study on cellular and cordless telephones and the risk for acoustic neuroma or meningioma in patients diagnosed 2000-2003. Neuroepidemiology2005; 25(3):120-128.
  • 30. Lönn S, Ahlbom A, Hall P, Feychting M. Long-term mobile phone use and brain tumor risk. Am J Epidemiol 2005; 161(6):526-535.
  • 31. Schoemaker M, Swerdlow A, Ahlbom A, Auvinen A, Blaasaas K, Cardis E, Christensen HC, Feychting M, Hepworth SJ, Johansen C, Klaeboe L, Lönn S, McKinney PA, Muir K, Raitanen J, Salminen T, Thomsen J, Tynes T. Mobile phone use and risk of acoustic neuroma: results of the Interphone casecontrol study in five North European countries. Br J Cancer 2005; 93(7):842-848.
  • 32. Hepworth S, Schoemaker M, Muir K, Swerdlow A, van Tongeren, MJA, McKinney PA. Mobile phone use and risk of glioma in adults: case-control study. BMJ 2006; 332(7546):883-887.
  • 33. Schüz J, Böhler E, Berg G, Schlehofer B, Hettinger I, Schlaefer K, Wahrendorf J, Kunna-Grass K, Blettner M. Cellular phones, cordless phones, and the risks of glioma and meningioma (Interphone Study Group, Germany). Am J Epidemiol 2006; 163(6):512-520.
  • 34. Klaeboe L, Blaasaas K, Tynes T. Use of mobile phones in Norway and risk of intracranial tumours. Eur J Cancer Prev 2007; 16(2):158-164.
  • 35. Takebayashi T, Akiba S, Kikuchi Y, Taki M, Wake K, Watanabe S, Yamaguchi N. Mobile phone use and acoustic neuroma risk in Japan. Occup Environ Med2006; 63(12):802-807.
  • 36. Lahkola A, Auvinen A, Raitanen J, Schoemaker M, Christensen H, Feychting M, Johansen C, Klaeboe L, Lönn S, Swerdlow AJ, Tynes T, Salminen T. Mobile phone use and risk of glioma in 5 North European countries. Int J Cancer 2007; 120(8):1769-1775.
  • 37. Schlehofer B, Schlaefer K, Blettner M, Berg G, Böhler E, Hettinger I, Kunna-Grass K, Wahrendorf J, Schüz J; Interphone Study Group. Environmental risk factors for sporadic acoustic neuroma (Interphone Study Group, Germany). Eur J Cancer 2007; 43(11):1741-1747.
  • 38. Hours M, Bernard M, Montestrucq L, Arslan M, Bergeret A, Deltour I, Cardis E. Cell phones and risk of brain and acoustic nerve tumours: the French INTERPHONE case-control study [in French]. Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique 2007; 55(5):321-332.
  • 39. Takebayashi T, Varsier N, Kikuchi Y, Wake K, Taki M, Watanabe S, Akiba S, Yamaguchi N. Mobile phone use, exposure to radiofrequency electromagnetic field, and brain tumour: a case-control study. Br J Cancer 2008; 98(3):652-659.
  • 40. Lahkola A, Salminen T, Raitanen J, Heinävaara S, Schoemaker M, Christensen HC, Feychting M, Johansen C, Klaeboe L, Lönn S, Swerdlow AJ, Tynes T, Auvinen A. Meningioma and mobile phone use-a collaborative case-control study in five North European countries. Int J Epidemiol 2008. 37(6):1304-1313.
  • 41. Mohyuddin A, Vokurka E, Evans D, Ramsden R, Jackson A. Is clinical growth index a reliable predictor of tumour growth in vestibular schwannomas? Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 2003; 28(2):85-90.
  • 42. Wrensch M, Minn Y, Chew T, Bondy M, Berger M. Epidemiology of primary brain tumors: current concepts and review of the literature. Neuro Oncol2002; 4(4):278-299.
  • 43. Kleihues P, Cavenee WK, editors. World Health Organization Classification of Tumours. Pathology and Genetics: Tumours of the Nervous System Lyon: International Agency for Research on Cancer; 2000.
  • 44. Schwartzbaum JA, Fisher JL, Aldape KD, Wrensch M. Epidemiology and molecular pathology of glioma. Nat Clin Pract Neurol 2006; 2(9):494-503.
  • 45. Umansky F, Shoshan Y, Rosnethal G, Fraifeld S, Spektor S. Radiation-induced meningioma. Neurosurg Focus [serial on the Internet] 2008;24(5):[about 1 p.]. Available from: http://thejns.org/doi/pdf/10.3171/FOC/2008/24/5/E7
  • 46. Kranzinger M, Jones N, Rittinger O, Pilz P, Piotrowski W, Manzl M, Galvan G, Kogelnik HD. Malignant glioma as a secondary malignant neoplasm after radiation therapy for cranio-pharyngioma: report of a case and review of reported cases. Onkologie 2001; 24(1):66-72.
  • 47. Sadetzki S, Chetrit A, Freedman L, Stovall M, Modan B, Novikov I. Long-term follow-up for brain tumor development after childhood exposure to ionizing radiation for tinea capitis. Radiat Res 2005; 163(4):424-432.
  • 48. Cahan W, Woodard H, Higinbotham N, Stewart F, Coley B. Sarcoma arising in irradiated bone. Report of eleven cases. Cancer 1948; 1:3-29.
  • 49. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. Pooled analysis of two case-control studies on use of cellular and cord-less telephones and the risk for malignant brain tumours diagnosed in 1997-2003. Int Arch Occup Environ Health2006; 79(8):630-639.
  • 50. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. Pooled analysis of two case-control studies on the use of cellular and cordless telephones and the risk of benign brain tumours diagnosed during 1997-2003. Int J Oncol 2006; 28(2):509-518.
  • 51. Cardis E, Richardson L, Deltour I, Armstrong B, Feychting M, Johansen C, Kilkenny M, McKinney P, Modan B, Sadetzki S, Schüz J, Swerdlow A, Vrijheid M, Auvinen A, Berg G, Blettner M, Bowman J, Brown J, Chetrit A, Christensen HC, Cook A, Hepworth S, Giles G, Hours M, Iavarone I, Jarus-Hakak A, Klaeboe L, Krewski D, Lagorio S, Lönn S, Mann S, McBride M, Muir K, Nadon L, Parent ME, Pearce N, Salminen T, Schoemaker M, Schlehofer B, Siemiatycki J, Taki M, Takebayashi T, Tynes T, van Tongeren M, Vecchia P, Wiart J, Woodward A, Yamaguchi N. The INTERPHONE study: design, epidemiological methods, and description of the study population. Eur J Epidemiol 2007; 22(9):647-664.
  • 52. Lönn S, Forssén U, Vecchia P, Ahlbom A, Feychting M. Output power levels from mobile phones in different geo-graphical areas; implications for exposure assessment. Occup Environ Med 2004; 61:769-772.
  • 53. Söderqvist F, Carlberg M, Hardell L. Use of wireless tele-phones and self-reported health symptoms: a population-based study among Swedish adolescents aged 15-19 years. Environ Health [serial on the Internet] 2008 [cited 2008 May 21]. Available from: http://www.ehjournal.net/content/pdf/1476069X-7-18.pdf
  • 54. Berg G, Schüz J, Samkange-Zeeb F, Blettner M. Assessment of radiofrequency exposure from cellular telephone daily use in an epidemiological study: German validation study of the international case-control study of cancers of the brain-INTERPHONE-study. J Expo Anal Environ Epidemiol 2005; 15(3):217-224.
  • 55. Vrijheid M, Cardis E, Armstrong B, Auvinen A, Berg G, Blaasaas K, Brown J, Carroll M, Chetrit A, Christensen HC, Deltour I, Feychting M, Giles GG, Hepworth SJ, Hours M, Iavarone I, Johansen C, Klaeboe L, Kurttio P, Lagorio S, Lönn S, McKinney PA, Montestrucq L, Parslow RC, Richardson L, Sadetzki S, Salminen T, Schüz J, Tynes T, Woodward A; Interphone Study Group. Validation of short term recall of mobile phone use for the Interphone study. Occup Environ Med 2006; 63(4):237-243.
  • 56. Vrijheid M, Deltour I, Krewski D, Sanchez M, Cardis E. The effects of recall errors and of selection bias in epi-demiologic studies of mobile phone use and cancer risk. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol 2006; 16(4):371-384.
  • 57. Hardell L, Hansson Mild K, Carlberg M, Söderqvist F. Tumour risk associated with use of cellular telephones or cordless desktop telephones. World J Surg Oncol 2006; 4:74-83.
  • 58. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern Epidemiology 3rd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2008.
  • 59. Kundi M, Hansson Mild K, Hardell L, Mattsson M. Mobile telephones and cancer-a review of epidemiological evi-dence. J Toxicol Environ Health B Crit Rev 2004; 7(5):351-384.
  • 60. Cardis E, Deltour I, Mann S, Moissonnier M, Taki M, Varsier N, Wake K, Wiart J. Distribution of RF energy emitted by mobile phones in anatomical structures of the brain. Phys Med Biol 2008; 53:2771-2783.
  • 61. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. Methodological aspects of epidemiological studies on the use of mobile phones and their association with brain tumors. Open Environ Sci 2008; 2:54-61.
  • 62. Lönn S. Mobile phone use and risk of intra-cranial tumors [doctoral thesis]. Stockholm: Karolinska Institutet; 2004.
  • 63. Chiabrera A, Bianco B, Moggia E, Kaufman J. Zeeman-Stark modeling of the RF EMF interaction with ligand binding. Bioelectromagnetics 2000; 21:312-324.
  • 64. Vanderstraeten J, Verschaeve L. Gene and protein expres-sion following exposure to radiofrequency fields from mobile phones. Environ Health Perspect2008; 116:1131-1135.
  • 65. Karinen A, Heinävaara S, Nylund R, Leszczynski D. Mobile phone radiation might alter protein expression in human skin. BMC Genomics [serial on the Internet] 2008 [cited 2008 Feb 11]. Available from: http://www.biomedcentral.com/content/pdf/14712164-9-77.pdf
  • 66. Lai H, Singh N. Melatonin and a spin-trap compound blocked radiofrequency radiation-induced DNA strand breaks in rat brain cells. Bioelectromagnetics 1997; 18:446-454.
  • 67. Phelan A, Lange D, Kues H, Lutty G. Modification of mem-brane fluidity in melanin containing cells by low level microwave radiation. Bioelectromagnetics 1992; 13:131-146.
  • 68. Simkó M, Hartwig C, Lantow M, Lupke M, Mattsson M, Rahman Q, Rollwitz J. Hsp70 expression and free radical release after exposure to non-thermal radio-frequency electromagnetic fields and ultrafine particles in human Mono Mac 6 cells. Toxicol Lett 2006; 161(1):73-82.
  • 69. Friedman J, Kraus S, Hauptman Y, Schiff Y, Seger R. Mechanism of short-term ERK activation by electro-magnetic fields at mobile phone frequencies. Biochem J 2007; 405(3):559-568.
  • 70. Berg G, Spallek J, Schüz J, Schlehofer B, Böhler E, Schlaefer K, Hettinger I, Kunna-Grass K, Wahrendorf J, Blettner M; Interphone Study Group, Germany. Occupational exposure to radio frequency/micro-wave radiation and the risk of brain tumors: Interphone Study Group, Germany. Am J Epidemiol 2006; 164(6):538-548.
  • 71. Grayson J. Radiation exposure socioeconomic status and brain tumor risk in the U.S. Air Force: a nested case-control study. Am J Epidemiol 1996; 143:480-486.
  • 72. Szmigielski S. Cancer morbidity in subjects occupationally exposed to high frequency (radiofrequency and microwave) electromagnetic radiation. Sci Total Environ 1996; 180:9-17.
  • 73. Thomas T, Stolley P, Stemhagen A, Fontham E, Bleeker M, Stewart P. Brain tumour mortality risk among men with electrical and electronic jobs: a case-control study. J Natl Cancer Inst 1987; 79:233-238.
  • 74. Hardell L, Carlberg M, Hansson Mild K. Use of cellular telephones and brain tumour risk in urban and rural areas. Occup Environ Med 2005; 62(6):390-394.
  • 75. Kundi M. Comments on the responses of Utteridge et al. (Radiat. Res. 159, 277-278, 2003) to letters about their paper (Radiat. Res. 158, 357-364, 2002). Radiat Res 2003; 160(5):613-614.
  • 76. Adey W, Byus C, Cain C, Higgins R, Jones R, Kean C, Kuster N, MacMurray A, Stagg RB, Zimmerman G. Spontaneous and nitrosourea-induced primary tumors of the central nervous system in Fischer 344 rats exposed to frequency-modulated microwave fields. Cancer Res 2000; 60(7):1857-1863.
  • 77. Hruby R, Neubauer G, Kuster N, Frauscher M. Study on potential effects of "902-MHz GSM-type Wireless Communication Signals" on DMBA-induced mammary tumours in Sprague-Dawley rats. Mutat Res 2008; 649:34-44.
  • 78. Repacholi M, Basten A, Gebski V, Noonan D, Finnie J, Harris A. Lymphomas in E mu-Pim1 transgenic mice exposed to pulsed 900 MHZ MHz electromagnetic fields. Radiat Res 1997; 147:631-640.
  • 79. Shirai T, Ichihara T, Wake K, Watanabe S, Yamanaka Y, Kawabe M, Taki M, Fujiwara O, Wang J, Takahashi S, Tamano S. Lack of promoting effects of chronic exposure to 1.95-GHz W-CDMA signals for IMT-2000 cellular system on development of N-ethylnitrosourea-induced central nervous system tumors in F344 rats. Bioelectromagnetics 2007; 28(7):562-572.
  • 80. Lai H, Singh N. Magnetic-field-induced DNA strand breaks in brain cells of the rat. Environ Health Perspect 2004; 112:687-694.
  • 81. Vijayalaxmi, Obe G. Controversial cytogenetic obser-vations in mammalian somatic cells exposed to radio-frequency radiation. Radiat Res 2004; 162:481-496.
  • 82. Vijayalaxmi, Prihoda TJ. Genetic damage in mammalian somatic cells exposed to radiofrequency radiation: a meta-analysis of data from 63 publications (1990-2005). Radiat Res 2008; 169(5):561-574.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    20 Aug 2010
  • Date of issue
    Aug 2010
ABRASCO - Associação Brasileira de Saúde Coletiva Av. Brasil, 4036 - sala 700 Manguinhos, 21040-361 Rio de Janeiro RJ - Brazil, Tel.: +55 21 3882-9153 / 3882-9151 - Rio de Janeiro - RJ - Brazil
E-mail: cienciasaudecoletiva@fiocruz.br