Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Evidence of changes in the oral language in children born full-term and small for gestational age: a systematic review

Abstract

Objective:

To perform a systematic review in order to verify the association between full-term birth of small for gestational age (SGA) children and the outcomes in the development of oral language.

Data source:

Articles from MEDLINE/PubMed, Web of Science, Embase, Lilacs, SciELO and Cochrane Library databases were identified, selected and critically evaluated by two independent reviewers and a judge, blindly, without language restriction and publication period. The PRISMA tool was used, and original studies with a theme involving children born full-term and SGA were included, outcome related to aspects of oral language development, as well as the use of tests, scales and/or specific questionnaires for the investigation, whose methodology was described in full, with children as the target population.

Data synthesis:

The researchers included nine articles based on the eligibility criteria. Studies have shown that being born SGA can interfere in aspects related to language and reported greater chances of under performance in SGA children when compared to children with appropriate size for gestational age. It was observed that the different studies did not have a uniform design, and the objectives were quite diverse. Furthermore, few of them had as focus issues related to the assessment of language, as well as the variability of instruments used to investigate this domain.

Conclusions:

The effects of low weight for gestation age in full-term infants continue beyond the neonatal period and may impact on children’s performance, mainly with regard to oral language development.

Keywords:
Infant, low birth weight; Infant, small for gestational age; Child language; Speech

Resumo

Objetivo:

Realizar uma revisão sistemática para verificar a associação entre o nascimento a termo de crianças pequenas para a idade gestacional (PIG) e os desfechos no desenvolvimento da linguagem oral.

Fontes de dados:

Artigos dos bancos de dados MEDLINE/PubMed, Web of Science, Embase, LILACS, SciELO e Cochrane Library foram identificados, selecionados e avaliados criticamente por dois revisores independentes e um juiz, às cegas, sem restrições de idioma e período de publicação. A ferramenta PRISMA foi utilizada e foram incluídos estudos originais envolvendo crianças nascidas a termo e PIG, desfechos relacionados a aspectos do desenvolvimento da linguagem oral, bem como o uso de testes, escalas e/ou questionários específicos para a investigação, cuja metodologia estava descrita na íntegra, com crianças como população-alvo.

Síntese dos dados:

Nove artigos foram incluídos a partir dos critérios de elegibilidade. Os estudos demonstraram que nascer PIG pode interferir em aspectos relacionados à linguagem e relataram que as chances de crianças PIG apresentarem um desempenho inferior são maiores quando comparadas as com tamanho adequado para a idade gestacional. Observou-se que os diferentes estudos não tinham um delineamento uniforme e seus objetivos eram bastante diversificados. Além disso, poucos focavam em questões relacionadas à avaliação da linguagem e foi possível notar uma variabilidade de instrumentos utilizados para investigar esse domínio.

Conclusões:

Os efeitos do baixo peso ao nascer em nascidos a termo persistem além do período neonatal e podem ter impacto no desempenho infantil, principalmente no que se refere ao desenvolvimento da linguagem oral.

Palavras-chave:
Recém-nascido de baixo peso; Recém-nascido pequeno para a idade gestacional; Linguagem infantil; Fala

INTRODUCTION

Proper development of the individual depends on factors related to pre-, peri- and postnatal life. Birth weight and ideal gestational age are considered prerequisites for favoring this development and, in addition to other biological and environmental factors, directly influence the future quality of life of the children.11. Chen H-J, Ko MH, Li S-T, Chiu N-C, Hung K-L. Prevalence of preschool children developmental disabilities in northeastern Taiwan - Screening with Taipei City Developmental Screening Checklist for Preschoolers, 2nd Version. J Formos Med Assoc. 2020;19:1174-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfma.2020.02.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfma.2020.02.0...
,22. Tu S, Wang A-L, Tan M-Z, Lu J-H, He J-R, Shen S-Y, et al. Family socioeconomic position and abnormal birth weight: evidence from a Chinese birth cohort. World J Pediatr. 2019;15:483-91. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12519-019-00279-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12519-019-00279...
Therefore, a careful look at children born full-term and small for gestational age (SGA) is necessary.

SGA children are those with birth weight below the expected for the respective gestational age in weeks. Therefore, it is more often considered as the weight below the 10th percentile, based on the intrauterine growth curve,33. Alexander GR, Kaufman RB, Himes JH, Mor J, Kogan M. A United States national reference for fetal growth. Obstet Gynecol. 1996;87:1-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/0029-7844(95)00386-x
https://doi.org/10.1016/0029-7844(95)003...
55. Villar J, Ismail LC, Victora CG, Ohuma EO, Bertino E, Altman DG, et al. International standards for newborn weight, length, and head circumference by gestational age and sex: the Newborn Cross-Sectional Study of the INTERGROWTH-21st Project. Lancet. 2014;384:857-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(14)60932-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(14)60...
and may be associated with intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR). Full-term are the ones born with gestational age between 37 and 41 weeks and 6 days, and when the weight is less than 2,500g, in addition to SGA, the literature classify them as underweight.66. World Health Organization. Standards for improving the quality of care for small and sick newborns in health facilities. Geneva: WHO; 2020.,77. World Health Organization. Child levels & trends in mortality report 2020. Geneva: WHO; 2020.,88. Black RE. Global Prevalence of small for gestational age births. Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser. 2015;81:1-7. https://doi.org/10.1159/000365790
https://doi.org/10.1159/000365790...

Studies have shown that the prevalence of live births with low weight can vary from 4 to 15%,99. Ruiz M, Goldblatt P, Morrison J, Kukla L, Švancara J, Riitta-Järvelin M, et al. Mother’s education and the risk of preterm and small for gestational age birth: a DRIVERS meta-analysis of 12 European cohorts. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2015;69:826-33. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2014-205387
https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2014-205387...
,1010. Buriol VC, Hirakata V, Goldani MZ, Silva CH. Temporal evolution of the risk factors associated with low birth weight rates in Brazilian capitals (1996-2011). Popul Health Metr. 2016;14:15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12963-016-0086-0
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12963-016-0086-...
with 3 to 4% full-term. In Brazil, this rate is 4.3%, which means more than 7 thousand children/year,1111. Brazil - Ministério da Saúde. DATASUS [homepage on the Internet]. Nascidos vivos na Bahia [cited 2019 May 16]. Brasília (DF): Ministério da Saúde; 2016. Available from: http://datasus.saude.gov.br/informacoes-de-saude
http://datasus.saude.gov.br/informacoes-...
a high number that must be considered in the elaboration of public policies and health promotion and prevention actions.

The causes of SGA birth and the risk factors related to it vary,1212. Chiavaroli V, Castorani V, Guidone P, Derraik JG, Liberati M, Chiarelli F, et al. Incidence of infants born small- and large-for-gestational-age in an Italian cohort over a 20-year period and associated risk factors. Ital J Pediatr. 2016;42:42. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-016-0254-7
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-016-0254-...
,1313. Tamura N, Hanaoka T, Ito K, Araki A, Miyashita C, Ito S, et al. Different risk factors for very low birth weight, term-small-for-gestational-age, or preterm birth in Japan. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15:369. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15020369
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15020369...
from genetic to environmental factors.1414. Matsuda N, Taki A, Tsuji A, Nakajima K, Takasawa K, Morioka C, et al. Perinatal factors affecting growth and development at age 3 years in extremely low birth weight infants born small for gestational age. Clin Pediatr Endocrinol. 2018;27:31-8. https://doi.org/10.1297/cpe.27.31
https://doi.org/10.1297/cpe.27.31...
Children born SGA have a higher risk of developing diseases such as obesity, coronary heart disease, high blood pressure, type 2 diabetes mellitus, dyslipidemia, delayed neuropsychomotor development, and visual, auditory, behavioral and learning problems,1515. Ewing AC, Ellington SR, Shapiro-Mendoza CK, Barfield WD, Kourtis AP. Full-term small-for-gestational-age newborns in the U.S.: characteristics, trends, and morbidity. Matern Child Health J. 2017;21:786-96. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-2165-z
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-2165-...
,1616. Vaag A. Low birth weight and early weight gain in the metabolic syndrome: consequences for infant nutrition. Int J Gynecol Obstet. 2009;104:S32-4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgo.2008.11.026
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgo.2008.11.0...
,1717. Rios NV, Alves C. Dificuldade escolar em crianças nascidas pequenas para a idade gestacional. Rev Ciênc Méd Biol. 2017;16:356. https://doi.org/10.9771/cmbio.v16i3.24369
https://doi.org/10.9771/cmbio.v16i3.2436...
,1818. Lundgren EM, Tuvemo T. Effects of being born small for gestational age on long-term intellectual performance. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2008;22:477-88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2008.01.014
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2008.01.0...
,1919. Sacchi C, Marino C, Nosarti C, Vieno A, Visentin S, Simonelli A. Association of Intrauterine growth restriction and small for gestational age status with childhood cognitive outcomes. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174:772. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1097
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2...
which directly interfere in the process of the speech and language development.2020. Rechia IC, Oliveira LD, Crestani AH, Biaggio EP, Souza AP. Effects of prematurity on language acquisition and auditory maturation: a systematic review. CoDAS. 2016;28:843-54. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015218
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201620...
,2121. Gouveia AS, Oliveira MM, Goulart AL, Azevedo MF, Perissinoto J. Development of speech and hearing skills in prematures adequate and small for gestational age: chronological age between 18 and 36 months. CoDAS. 2020;32:e20180275. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20192018275
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201920...
,2222. Souza AC, Casais-e-Silva LL, Sena EP. The influence of prematurity on the development of phonological skills. Rev CEFAC. 2019;21:1-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/201921413118
https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/201921...
Therefore, the relationship between complications during this process and the low weight of children born full-term needs more research to find more conclusions.

In previous reviews,2020. Rechia IC, Oliveira LD, Crestani AH, Biaggio EP, Souza AP. Effects of prematurity on language acquisition and auditory maturation: a systematic review. CoDAS. 2016;28:843-54. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015218
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201620...
,2323. Zerbeto AB, Cortelo FM, Filho ÉB. Association between gestational age and birth weight on the language development of Brazilian children: a systematic review. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2015;91:326-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jped.2014.11.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jped.2014.11.0...
studies took into account low weight and prematurity. In the present article, the authors assessed the low weight of children born full-term classified as SGA and hypothesized that a large part of full-term children and SGA are susceptible to presenting changes in the development of language skills.

However, this is not a topic with a comprehensive approach in the specialized literature, whose research has diversified designs. Moreover, data on these skills are little explored and often inconclusive, generating important questions regarding the dimension of the altered linguistic aspects.

Therefore, this investigation focused on verifying findings in the literature that specify the association between the term birth of SGA children and the outcomes in the development of oral language.

METHOD

The present investigation considered the PICO structure2424. O’Connor D, Green S, Higgins JP, editors. Chapter 5: Defining the review question and developing criteria for including studies. In: Higgins JP, Green S, editors. Cochrane Handbook of Systematic Reviews of Intervention. Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470712184.ch5
https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470712184.ch...
to describe the components related to the identified problem and to structure the following research question: Is there evidence that changes in oral language may be present in SGA children born full-term?

The selection of articles was executed in the electronic databases independently and blindly, using the Mesh descriptors previously defined. Each reviewer, separately, judged the inclusion of articles based on reading the titles and, when available, the abstracts. After the first stage, the articles were selected and thus they were read in full to confirm eligibility and inclusion in the study. In a third moment, the results found were confronted among two reviewers, and the disagreements were resolved through a judge (third reviewer), with expertise in systematic review.

The researchers included original studies with a theme involving children born full-term and SGA, regardless of the reference curve used. These studies needed to have some outcome related to the occurrence of problems in the development of oral language. Furthermore, the use of tests, scales and/or questionnaires specific to the investigation were also included in this article. Studies whose methodology was fully described, with a target population of children (aged between two and ten years), without language restrictions and period of publication, with texts available in full, were prioritized.

The researchers excluded duplicate articles, literature review, case reports or case series, studies that evaluated aspects of children’s language exclusively in children born with low weight and premature, or that did not relate the gestational age of the evaluated SGA population.

The systematic literature review was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyzes (PRISMA) methodology.2525. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.100...
The selection of the studies was made based on the titles found initially and, after being selected, the abstracts were read. In cases in which reading the abstract was not sufficient to establish whether the article should be incorporated, the text was read in full to determine its eligibility. When abstracts were sufficient, full versions were selected to confirm eligibility and inclusion in the study.

The search strategy was based on the electronic databases: MEDLINE/PubMed, Web of Science, Embase, Lilacs, SciELO and Cochrane Library. The articles were identified between July 2019 and January 2020. The descriptors used as a search strategy were: low birth weight, LBW, Small for Gestational Age, SGA, language, speech, Speech-Language Pathology, combined through Boolean operators OR and/or AND, resulting in the search details as follows: “low birth weight” <OR> “LBW” <OR> “Small for Gestational Age” <OR> “SGA” AND language * OR speech * OR speech language pathology.

The criteria proposed by the Effective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP — Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies)2626. Effective Public Health Practice Project. Quality assessment tool for quantitative studies. Eff Public Heal Pract Proj. 2010;2-5.,2727. Thomas BH, Ciliska D, Dobbins M, Micucci S. A process for systematically reviewing the literature: providing the research evidence for public health nursing interventions. Worldviews Evidence-Based Nurs. 2004;1:176-84. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1524-475x.2004.04006.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1524-475x.2004...
were used in order to assess the quality of the evidence and methodological of the studies, especially the details regarding the selection bias, study design, potential confounders, blindness of researchers and participants, data collection methods (if they were valid and reliable), losses follow-up (exclusion or loss of follow-up), integrity of the intervention and appropriate analysis of the research question. According to the definition of each of these criteria, the studies were then classified as poor quality (presence of two or more weak items), moderate (presence of only one weak item) or strong (absence of weak items).

As for the quality of scientific writing, the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE)2828. Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Int J Surg. 2014;12:1495-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2014.07.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2014.07.0...
elaboration guide was adopted in order to verify the accuracy of scientific writing in the selected studies. The quality index of each article corresponded to the sum of the total number of items assessed as positive, with the maximum score being 22 (100%). Articles with 50% or more agreed with criteria considered to be of regular quality and those with more than 75%, of good quality.

The analysis of the studies found was performed in a descriptive manner and executed in three stages. The first included the description of the following methodological characteristics: authorship, country/year of publication, study design, objective, sample, test or protocol used to assess aspects of language. The second stage comprised the analysis of the outcome and the factors associated with it, that is, research instruments, main results with description of aspects of the target audience’s language, conclusion and EPHPP and STROBE score. The third step consisted of analyzing the aspects of language addressed in each text, considering the linguistic subsystems pragmatic, morphosyntactic, lexical and phonological and other cognitive aspects. The results were presented as a narrative synthesis of the existing literature that relates the term SGA birth and aspects of children’s language.

RESULTS

Figure 1 illustrates the selection process of the articles that composed the present review. Of the total, 2,613 articles were excluded because they did not meet the required inclusion criteria; however, nine articles were included based on the eligibility criteria.

Figure 1.
Flowchart of article selection based on PRISMA criteria.

As shown in Table 1 of the selected studies, one was done in Brazil2929. Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x200300...
and eight were published internationally. The sample number of studies ranged from 1029 to 3,73830 full-term SGA children.

Table 1.
General characteristics of the studies included in the systematic review.

Regarding the SGA classification, a Japanese study considered birth weight below the tenth percentile for gestational age (GA) and birth length below -2.0 standard deviation (SD) for GA, or birth weight below -2.0 SD for GA and length below the tenth percentile for GA.3131. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Nakamura K, Tamai K, Mori S, Nakamura M, et al. Catch-up growth and neurobehavioral development among full-term, small-for-gestational-age children: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. J Pediatr. 2018;192:41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09....
In the other studies2929. Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x200300...
,3030. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015....
,3232. O’Neill SM, Hannon G, Khashan AS, Hourihane JO, Kenny LC, Kiely M, et al. Thin-for-gestational age infants are at increased risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 2 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017;102:F197-202. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-310791
https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-201...
3737. Castro Conde JR, Campo CG, González NL, Millán B, Barrios D, Sosa A, et al. Assessment of neonatal EEG background and neurodevelopment in full-term small for their gestational age infants. Pediatr Res. 2019;88:91-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-...
only birth weight below the tenth percentile for GA was considered according to local standards, based on the uterine growth curve.33. Alexander GR, Kaufman RB, Himes JH, Mor J, Kogan M. A United States national reference for fetal growth. Obstet Gynecol. 1996;87:1-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/0029-7844(95)00386-x
https://doi.org/10.1016/0029-7844(95)003...
,55. Villar J, Ismail LC, Victora CG, Ohuma EO, Bertino E, Altman DG, et al. International standards for newborn weight, length, and head circumference by gestational age and sex: the Newborn Cross-Sectional Study of the INTERGROWTH-21st Project. Lancet. 2014;384:857-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(14)60932-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(14)60...
Some authors3030. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015....
reported that they disregarded height due to inaccuracies in the measurements at birth, following the evidence.44. Arcangeli T, Thilaganathan B, Hooper R, Khan KS, Bhide A. Neurodevelopmental delay in small babies at term: a systematic review. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2012;40:267-75. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112...

It was found that only one article was classified as having poor quality of evidence, while the others were considered to have moderate to strong evidence according to the EPHPP criteria. The article considered weak also had a small sample size.2929. Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x200300...
As for the quality of scientific writing, one article was considered of regular quality2929. Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x200300...
and the others were classified as of good quality (Table 2).

Table 2.
Characteristics of the studies included in the systematic review of changes in aspects of oral language of children born full-term small for gestational age.

In the reviewed studies, there was a diversity of objectives, with few being directed to the evaluation of some aspect of language.2929. Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x200300...
,3535. Klarić A, Kolundžić Z, Galić S, Bošnjak V. Language development in preschool children born after asymmetrical intrauterine growth retardation. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2012;16:132-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.0...
In the other selected studies,3030. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015....
3434. Savchev S, Sanz-Cortes M, Cruz-Martinez R, Arranz A, Botet F, Gratacos E, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome of full-term small-for-gestational-age infants with normal placental function. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2013;42:201-6. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391...
,3636. Walker SP, Chang SM, Younger N, Grantham-Mcgregor SM. The effect of psychosocial stimulation on cognition and behaviour at 6 years in a cohort of term, low-birthweight Jamaican children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52:e148-54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03637.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010...
,3737. Castro Conde JR, Campo CG, González NL, Millán B, Barrios D, Sosa A, et al. Assessment of neonatal EEG background and neurodevelopment in full-term small for their gestational age infants. Pediatr Res. 2019;88:91-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-...
this ability was described as being part of the tests that assessed cognitive skills and neurobehavioral aspects of development.

Table 3 shows that the Bayley scale was the instrument used in four analyzed studies. It was used the average age group of two years to perform the assessment.3232. O’Neill SM, Hannon G, Khashan AS, Hourihane JO, Kenny LC, Kiely M, et al. Thin-for-gestational age infants are at increased risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 2 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017;102:F197-202. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-310791
https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-201...
3434. Savchev S, Sanz-Cortes M, Cruz-Martinez R, Arranz A, Botet F, Gratacos E, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome of full-term small-for-gestational-age infants with normal placental function. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2013;42:201-6. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391...
,3737. Castro Conde JR, Campo CG, González NL, Millán B, Barrios D, Sosa A, et al. Assessment of neonatal EEG background and neurodevelopment in full-term small for their gestational age infants. Pediatr Res. 2019;88:91-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-...
In other studies,3030. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015....
,3131. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Nakamura K, Tamai K, Mori S, Nakamura M, et al. Catch-up growth and neurobehavioral development among full-term, small-for-gestational-age children: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. J Pediatr. 2018;192:41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09....
aspects of language were mentioned, but they used questionnaires directed at parents who investigated issues related to neurodevelopment and behavior as instruments.

Table 3.
Description of the research instruments, and their respective methods of application, in studies that assessed language skills.

In the study3535. Klarić A, Kolundžić Z, Galić S, Bošnjak V. Language development in preschool children born after asymmetrical intrauterine growth retardation. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2012;16:132-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.0...
it was confirmed the use of tests that contemplated various aspects of language. Authors3636. Walker SP, Chang SM, Younger N, Grantham-Mcgregor SM. The effect of psychosocial stimulation on cognition and behaviour at 6 years in a cohort of term, low-birthweight Jamaican children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52:e148-54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03637.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010...
carried out a research in children with LBW, born full-term, to verify whether psychosocial stimulation, up to the age of 2 years, benefited the development of cognition and behavioral aspects at 6 years of age. The sample of SGA children was divided into an intervention group, which received stimulation for two years, and another control group, which did not receive it, and compared it with appropriate size for gestational age (AGA) children. The receptive vocabulary was assessed using the PPVT image test (Pea-body Picture Vocabulary Test), the only skill analyzed in the study related directly to aspects of language. Other aspects, such as memory, attention, reading ability and behavior, were also evaluated.

It was observed that the received intervention contributed for the group to present better performances in the aspects evaluated at 6 years of age, however there were no significant differences mainly in the assessment of linguistic ability.

Table 4 shows the aspects of language that were possibly analyzed in each study. These aspects were selected from the analysis of the instrument or technique applied in the studies. It was noted that data related to semantics and morphosyntax were the most analyzed in the reviewed texts.

Table 4.
Aspects of children’s language analyzed in the included studies.

DISCUSSION

According to what was observed, the studies showed that SGA birth can interfere in aspects related to language and reported higher chances of underperformance in SGA children when compared to AGA children.

These results, however, must be interpreted with caution, since the different studies did not have a uniform design, the objectives were quite diverse and few had as focus issues related to the assessment of linguistic skills, in addition to the variability of instruments used to investigate that domain.

It was found that studies with full-term SGA children are not frequent, especially when it relates to aspects of development including language. Studies with this population have important limitations, such as different assessment instruments; small, heterogeneous and sometimes non-representative samples of the population; precarious detailing of clinical and sociodemographic characteristics, among others. For this reason, the two studies3030. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015....
,3131. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Nakamura K, Tamai K, Mori S, Nakamura M, et al. Catch-up growth and neurobehavioral development among full-term, small-for-gestational-age children: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. J Pediatr. 2018;192:41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09....
that used a questionnaire directed to parents were not excluded.

Even considering the absence of child evaluations as a limitation, preventing more targeted conclusions, these studies3030. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015....
,3131. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Nakamura K, Tamai K, Mori S, Nakamura M, et al. Catch-up growth and neurobehavioral development among full-term, small-for-gestational-age children: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. J Pediatr. 2018;192:41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09....
arouse the scientific environment for the investigation of outcomes related to the development of speech and language since at least two questions of the instruments used were about linguistic aspects. Screenings, as those used by the authors, may serve to identify children at risk for aspects of neurodevelopment, even though it is subjective because it is based on their parents’ opinions. Therefore, this perception is of fundamental importance and often contributes to the early diagnosis process.

The findings revealed that SGA birth was a risk factor for developmental delay among children who were born full-term, corroborating with other studies.44. Arcangeli T, Thilaganathan B, Hooper R, Khan KS, Bhide A. Neurodevelopmental delay in small babies at term: a systematic review. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2012;40:267-75. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112...
,3333. Simões RV, Cruz-Lemini M, Bargalló N, Gratacós E, Sanz-Cortés M. Brain metabolite differences in one-year-old infants born small at term and association with neurodevelopmental outcome. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2015;213:210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.04.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.04.0...
,3434. Savchev S, Sanz-Cortes M, Cruz-Martinez R, Arranz A, Botet F, Gratacos E, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome of full-term small-for-gestational-age infants with normal placental function. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2013;42:201-6. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391...
The authors3030. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015....
,3131. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Nakamura K, Tamai K, Mori S, Nakamura M, et al. Catch-up growth and neurobehavioral development among full-term, small-for-gestational-age children: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. J Pediatr. 2018;192:41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09....
highlighted the importance of continuous monitoring, in order to detect behavioral problems and provide appropriate interventions to SGA children, especially those with failed growth speed (catch up). The authors of this review questioned the other aspects of language that were not addressed and the lack of more precise conclusions related to the development of speech and language.

It is noteworthy that most of the texts analyzed and included in this review related the SGA birth to other clinical states and their effects on child development, among them, some linguistic aspects. It was noticed that language was one of the aspects evaluated within cognitive skills, being analyzed specifically in a few studies.2929. Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x200300...
,3535. Klarić A, Kolundžić Z, Galić S, Bošnjak V. Language development in preschool children born after asymmetrical intrauterine growth retardation. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2012;16:132-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.0...

Linguistic aspects were related because it is part of children’s neurobehavioral development. Authors3838. Wong HS, Santhakumaran S, Cowan FM, Modi N. Developmental assessments in preterm children: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2016;138:e20160251. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-0251
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-0251...
,3939. Vohr B. Speech and language outcomes of very preterm infants. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014;19:78-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10.0...
,4040. Santos ES, Kieviet JF, Königs M, Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J. Predictive value of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development on development of very preterm/very low birth weight children: a meta-analysis. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89:487-96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.03.008
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.201...
already mentioned the importance of evaluating these aspects throughout development. The Bayley III scale was the instrument used in four studies.

The use of the scale makes it possible to identify and quantify developmental delay, but longitudinal assessments of the child are essential to complete any type of change and enable the necessary referrals for therapeutic interventions to minimize future side-effects.

In the three studies in this review that used the scale,3333. Simões RV, Cruz-Lemini M, Bargalló N, Gratacós E, Sanz-Cortés M. Brain metabolite differences in one-year-old infants born small at term and association with neurodevelopmental outcome. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2015;213:210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.04.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.04.0...
,3434. Savchev S, Sanz-Cortes M, Cruz-Martinez R, Arranz A, Botet F, Gratacos E, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome of full-term small-for-gestational-age infants with normal placental function. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2013;42:201-6. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391...
,3737. Castro Conde JR, Campo CG, González NL, Millán B, Barrios D, Sosa A, et al. Assessment of neonatal EEG background and neurodevelopment in full-term small for their gestational age infants. Pediatr Res. 2019;88:91-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-...
a lower performance was found in the assessed domains in the SGA group when compared to the AGA group, as it was not observed in the study by O’Neill et al.3232. O’Neill SM, Hannon G, Khashan AS, Hourihane JO, Kenny LC, Kiely M, et al. Thin-for-gestational age infants are at increased risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 2 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017;102:F197-202. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-310791
https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-201...

When children born full-term SGA were evaluated at 2 years of age, authors also obtained lower scores in the mentioned domains of the scale.3434. Savchev S, Sanz-Cortes M, Cruz-Martinez R, Arranz A, Botet F, Gratacos E, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome of full-term small-for-gestational-age infants with normal placental function. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2013;42:201-6. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391...
In the study, the result of neurodevelopment at 2 years of term SGA newborns with and without Doppler changes in the umbilical artery was evaluated, ruling out placental dysfunction. Even without initially presenting a placental dysfunction that justifies SGA birth, the study highlighted that babies are in need of a more differentiated look at development, as there is evidence of delays and may be suggestive of interrupted neurological maturation during pregnancy.

The SGA children that were evaluated in a study3737. Castro Conde JR, Campo CG, González NL, Millán B, Barrios D, Sosa A, et al. Assessment of neonatal EEG background and neurodevelopment in full-term small for their gestational age infants. Pediatr Res. 2019;88:91-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-...
in which the objective was to quantify the rates of immature neonatal electroencephalogram (EEG) patterns and associate them with neurodevelopment were diagnosed with IUGR between the second and third trimester by Doppler and biometric measurements on fetal ultrasound, and confirmed with birth weight <tenth percentile. The authors found a significant correlation between interhemispheric asymmetry and lower scores on motor and language development.

IUGR appeared as one of the factors associated with SGA birth. Authors44. Arcangeli T, Thilaganathan B, Hooper R, Khan KS, Bhide A. Neurodevelopmental delay in small babies at term: a systematic review. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2012;40:267-75. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112...
,4141. Levine TA, Grunau RE, McAuliffe FM, Pinnamaneni R, Foran A, Alderdice FA. Early Childhood Neurodevelopment After Intrauterine Growth Restriction: a Systematic Review. Pediatrics. 2015;135:126-41. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-1143
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-1143...
,4242. Miller SL, Huppi PS, Mallard C. The consequences of fetal growth restriction on brain structure and neurodevelopmental outcome. J Physiol. 2016;594:807-23. https://doi.org/10.1113/jp271402
https://doi.org/10.1113/jp271402...
,4343. Alves TL, Ribeiro HC, Costa ML, Valois SS. Newborn risk factors according to gestational age: a review. Nutrire Rev Soc Bras Aliment Nutr. 2015;40:376-82.,4444. Figueras F, Gardosi J. Intrauterine growth restriction: new concepts in antenatal surveillance, diagnosis, and management. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2011;204:288-300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2010.08.055
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2010.08.0...
revealed that not all SGA babies are pathologically small and there are several reasons for SGA birth, such as gestational age at birth, ethnicity, parents’ stature, presence of fetal abnormalities, fetal exposure to alcohol or drugs, and maternal diseases. Studies44. Arcangeli T, Thilaganathan B, Hooper R, Khan KS, Bhide A. Neurodevelopmental delay in small babies at term: a systematic review. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2012;40:267-75. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112
https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112...
,1919. Sacchi C, Marino C, Nosarti C, Vieno A, Visentin S, Simonelli A. Association of Intrauterine growth restriction and small for gestational age status with childhood cognitive outcomes. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174:772. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1097
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2...
,4545. Figueras F, Oros D, Cruz-Martinez R, Padilla N, Hernandez-Andrade E, Botet F, et al. Neurobehavior in term, small-for-gestational age infants with normal placental function. Pediatrics. 2009;124:e934-41. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3346
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3346...
showed that full-term SGA children with or without IUGR had lower scores in the formal assessment of neurodevelopment. It is believed that, since the language is an intrinsic skill and dependent on these aspects, it is possible to present atypical development in full-term SGA children.

Two revised texts3535. Klarić A, Kolundžić Z, Galić S, Bošnjak V. Language development in preschool children born after asymmetrical intrauterine growth retardation. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2012;16:132-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.0...
,3737. Castro Conde JR, Campo CG, González NL, Millán B, Barrios D, Sosa A, et al. Assessment of neonatal EEG background and neurodevelopment in full-term small for their gestational age infants. Pediatr Res. 2019;88:91-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-...
evaluated SGA children with IUGR. The results showed that children with IUGR had worse language results compared to the control group. These difficulties were present in the comprehension of language, content, structure and in the reduced size of the vocabulary that compromises the comprehension and expression of the language. These processes occur in frontotemporal areas; therefore, they suggest that any compromise in the volume and structure of this area can contribute to difficulties in understanding.

Asymmetric IUGR affects the frontal cortex neural networks, with direct implications for learning and memory functions,4646. Geva R, Eshel R, Leitner Y, Valevski AF, Harel S. Neuropsychological outcome of children with intrauterine growth restriction: a 9-year prospective study. Pediatrics. 2006;118:91-100. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2343
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2343...
,4747. Geva R, Eshel R, Leitner Y, Fattal-Valevski A, Harel S. Memory functions of children born with asymmetric intrauterine growth restriction. Brain Res. 2006;1117:186-94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006....
and, as suggested by authors,4848. Kisilevsky BS, Davies GA. Auditory processing deficits in growth restricted fetuses affect later language development. Med Hypotheses. 2007;68:620-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2006.08.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2006.08.0...
impairments also in auditory processing that directly interferes with speech perception.

In this study, it was observed that the children presented lower results in the phonological coding and decoding tests, which are important for the process of learning to read and write. It is understood that losses in these skills may imply difficulties mainly with reading.4949. Wile TL, Borowsky R. What does rapid automatized naming measure? A new RAN task compared to naming and lexical decision. Brain Lang. 2004;90:47-62. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00419-x
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00...
,5050. Mousinho R, Alves LM, Luiza NA, Salgado-Azoni CA, Celeste LC, Capelini SA, et al. Leitura, escrita e matemática: do desenvolvimento aos transtornos específicos da aprendizagem. Instituto ABCD; 2020. The authors3535. Klarić A, Kolundžić Z, Galić S, Bošnjak V. Language development in preschool children born after asymmetrical intrauterine growth retardation. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2012;16:132-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.0...
conclude that IUGR has a negative impact on language development, which is evident in pre-school age. As a limitation of the study, the authors pointed out the difficulty of differentiating babies who actually had IUGR and those who were just SGA for other reasons.

It is observed that semantics and morphosyntax are the language skills most analyzed in the studies in this review. In five of these studies,3232. O’Neill SM, Hannon G, Khashan AS, Hourihane JO, Kenny LC, Kiely M, et al. Thin-for-gestational age infants are at increased risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 2 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017;102:F197-202. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-310791
https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-201...
3535. Klarić A, Kolundžić Z, Galić S, Bošnjak V. Language development in preschool children born after asymmetrical intrauterine growth retardation. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2012;16:132-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.0...
,3737. Castro Conde JR, Campo CG, González NL, Millán B, Barrios D, Sosa A, et al. Assessment of neonatal EEG background and neurodevelopment in full-term small for their gestational age infants. Pediatr Res. 2019;88:91-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-...
some scales were used that generally contemplate all aspects of oral language, in addition to other skills related to development. In view of the scientific and methodological quality of the five texts reviewed, it is believed that these results are efficient, showing lower performance of the SGA group in the linguistic domains evaluated when compared to the AGA group, except in one study.3232. O’Neill SM, Hannon G, Khashan AS, Hourihane JO, Kenny LC, Kiely M, et al. Thin-for-gestational age infants are at increased risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 2 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017;102:F197-202. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-310791
https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-201...
In the other texts that included semantic and morphosyntactic aspects, in at least two of them,2929. Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x200300...
,3030. Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015....
the data were little explored and methodological flaws were observed, however they pointed out that being born full-term and SGA are risk factors for possible language development delays.

It was found that pragmatics and phonology were the language skills with the lowest percentage of analysis. It is understood that the understanding of what is spoken anticipates the expression. The pragmatic and semantic subsystems are the first to be observed in child development. The communicative exchanges supported by vocal, verbal and non-verbal means reveal the limited linguistic capacity, preventing the correct production of the sounds of the language and the structuring of more complex phrases.5151. Andrade CR, Béfi-lopes DM, Fernandes FD, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de Linguagem Infantil nas áreas de Fonologia, Vocabulário, Fluência e Pragmática. São Paulo: Prófono; 2004.,5252. Monteiro-Luperi TI, Befi-Lopes DM, Diniz EM, Krebs VL, Carvalho WB. Linguistic performance in 2 years old preterm, considering chronological age and corrected age. CoDAS. 2016;28:118-22. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015075
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201620...

The conventional use of the oral language develops with the appearance of the first words, followed by the production of simple phrases, followed by the complex ones, until reaching proficiency and becoming a native speaker, with the phonological system completely acquired. This process is complex and it involves several factors that can directly interfere with the child’s linguistic performance, such as neurocognitive, auditory-perceptual, linguistic, individual, interactional, environmental and sociocultural.5353. Brancalioni AR, Zauza A, Karlinski CD, Quitaiski LF, Thomaz MF. Expressive vocabulary performance of students aged from 4 to 5 years attending public and private schools. Audiol Commun Res. 2018;23:1-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2016-1836
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2016-1...
,5454. Bernthal J, Bankson NW, Flipsen PJ. Articulation and phonological disorders: speech sound disorders in children. 8th ed. New York: PEARSON; 2017.

Some authors5555. Silva IB, Lindau TA, Giacheti CM. Preschool preterm infants’ oral language assessment instruments: a literature review. Rev CEFAC. 2017;19:90-8. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201719112416
https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-02162017191...
cited instruments used in the assessment of the spoken language of premature preschoolers. It was found that, of the eight, six were international instruments, most of them development scales. Besides that, Brazilian researchers built two of them. It is believed that there is a lack of tests considered gold standard with methodological and scientific rigor, not only in Brazil but also in other countries.

It is noticed that the scores of the normative processes for the population that applies, when they exist, were obtained through validation with unrepresentative numbers and reduced numbers of subjects. The importance of studies in this area is emphasized, using the most appropriate methodology, covering all linguistic aspects, and therefore producing more consistent data.

It can be concluded that the effects of low weight continue beyond the neonatal period and can have an impact on the child’s performance, especially in regard with issues related to language.

This finding is of great relevance for the competent bodies to implement public policies aimed to this population, such as early diagnosis and intervention programs, in addition to drawing the attention of health professionals and family members who must remain alert to any changes in development. The language assessment of children born full-term and SGA, as well as the monitoring in the early and school phases, can prevent future learning problems and favor aspects related to mental health by avoiding losses in the development of speech and language, which ends up being a high cost problem for the individual and society.

An important political issue in our country, considering resource constraints, is to decide whether interventions in child development should only be for children considered at risk, and this mainly relates to prematurity, or also, for those considered to be SGA, as the evidence of this review underline the need to reach children with low weight, including those born full-term. There is an important gap related to this topic and its intention is to encourage future research.

In view of the possibilities of changes in the development of children’s language in this population, the scarcity of studies that assess linguistic skills and monitor development, as well as the lack of interest on the part of the existing literature, to delve deeper into the investigations of such aspects, are emphasized once again. Futhermore, some limitations need to be considered. The lack of standardization in the tests and the reduced number of subjects, added to the heterogeneity of the tests and analysis of the results, made interpretation difficult, as well as the generalization of the results, and made it impossible to perform meta-analyzes. Another limitation was that the researched gestational age, which restricted the number of selected articles. The vast majority of articles researched related to low weight, focused on studying prematurity.

It is essential to properly assess the development of children born low weight and full-term, since they are also susceptible to changes in development and these are more prevalent in relation to those who were born suitable for gestational age.

In conclusion, the nine articles selected from the eligibility criteria pointed out that being born full-term and SGA can interfere in aspects related to language. The effects of low weight on SGA and full-term children continue beyond the neonatal period and can have an impact on children’s performance, especially in regard to issues related to the development of oral language.

  • Funding
    Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado da Bahia. FAPESB. Brasil. Processo n0: 4771/2017.

REFERENCES

  • 1.
    Chen H-J, Ko MH, Li S-T, Chiu N-C, Hung K-L. Prevalence of preschool children developmental disabilities in northeastern Taiwan - Screening with Taipei City Developmental Screening Checklist for Preschoolers, 2nd Version. J Formos Med Assoc. 2020;19:1174-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfma.2020.02.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfma.2020.02.001
  • 2.
    Tu S, Wang A-L, Tan M-Z, Lu J-H, He J-R, Shen S-Y, et al. Family socioeconomic position and abnormal birth weight: evidence from a Chinese birth cohort. World J Pediatr. 2019;15:483-91. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12519-019-00279-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12519-019-00279-7
  • 3.
    Alexander GR, Kaufman RB, Himes JH, Mor J, Kogan M. A United States national reference for fetal growth. Obstet Gynecol. 1996;87:1-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/0029-7844(95)00386-x
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0029-7844(95)00386-x
  • 4.
    Arcangeli T, Thilaganathan B, Hooper R, Khan KS, Bhide A. Neurodevelopmental delay in small babies at term: a systematic review. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2012;40:267-75. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.11112
  • 5.
    Villar J, Ismail LC, Victora CG, Ohuma EO, Bertino E, Altman DG, et al. International standards for newborn weight, length, and head circumference by gestational age and sex: the Newborn Cross-Sectional Study of the INTERGROWTH-21st Project. Lancet. 2014;384:857-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(14)60932-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(14)60932-6
  • 6.
    World Health Organization. Standards for improving the quality of care for small and sick newborns in health facilities. Geneva: WHO; 2020.
  • 7.
    World Health Organization. Child levels & trends in mortality report 2020. Geneva: WHO; 2020.
  • 8.
    Black RE. Global Prevalence of small for gestational age births. Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser. 2015;81:1-7. https://doi.org/10.1159/000365790
    » https://doi.org/10.1159/000365790
  • 9.
    Ruiz M, Goldblatt P, Morrison J, Kukla L, Švancara J, Riitta-Järvelin M, et al. Mother’s education and the risk of preterm and small for gestational age birth: a DRIVERS meta-analysis of 12 European cohorts. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2015;69:826-33. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2014-205387
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2014-205387
  • 10.
    Buriol VC, Hirakata V, Goldani MZ, Silva CH. Temporal evolution of the risk factors associated with low birth weight rates in Brazilian capitals (1996-2011). Popul Health Metr. 2016;14:15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12963-016-0086-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s12963-016-0086-0
  • 11.
    Brazil - Ministério da Saúde. DATASUS [homepage on the Internet]. Nascidos vivos na Bahia [cited 2019 May 16]. Brasília (DF): Ministério da Saúde; 2016. Available from: http://datasus.saude.gov.br/informacoes-de-saude
    » http://datasus.saude.gov.br/informacoes-de-saude
  • 12.
    Chiavaroli V, Castorani V, Guidone P, Derraik JG, Liberati M, Chiarelli F, et al. Incidence of infants born small- and large-for-gestational-age in an Italian cohort over a 20-year period and associated risk factors. Ital J Pediatr. 2016;42:42. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-016-0254-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-016-0254-7
  • 13.
    Tamura N, Hanaoka T, Ito K, Araki A, Miyashita C, Ito S, et al. Different risk factors for very low birth weight, term-small-for-gestational-age, or preterm birth in Japan. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15:369. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15020369
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15020369
  • 14.
    Matsuda N, Taki A, Tsuji A, Nakajima K, Takasawa K, Morioka C, et al. Perinatal factors affecting growth and development at age 3 years in extremely low birth weight infants born small for gestational age. Clin Pediatr Endocrinol. 2018;27:31-8. https://doi.org/10.1297/cpe.27.31
    » https://doi.org/10.1297/cpe.27.31
  • 15.
    Ewing AC, Ellington SR, Shapiro-Mendoza CK, Barfield WD, Kourtis AP. Full-term small-for-gestational-age newborns in the U.S.: characteristics, trends, and morbidity. Matern Child Health J. 2017;21:786-96. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-2165-z
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-2165-z
  • 16.
    Vaag A. Low birth weight and early weight gain in the metabolic syndrome: consequences for infant nutrition. Int J Gynecol Obstet. 2009;104:S32-4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgo.2008.11.026
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgo.2008.11.026
  • 17.
    Rios NV, Alves C. Dificuldade escolar em crianças nascidas pequenas para a idade gestacional. Rev Ciênc Méd Biol. 2017;16:356. https://doi.org/10.9771/cmbio.v16i3.24369
    » https://doi.org/10.9771/cmbio.v16i3.24369
  • 18.
    Lundgren EM, Tuvemo T. Effects of being born small for gestational age on long-term intellectual performance. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2008;22:477-88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2008.01.014
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2008.01.014
  • 19.
    Sacchi C, Marino C, Nosarti C, Vieno A, Visentin S, Simonelli A. Association of Intrauterine growth restriction and small for gestational age status with childhood cognitive outcomes. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174:772. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1097
    » https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.1097
  • 20.
    Rechia IC, Oliveira LD, Crestani AH, Biaggio EP, Souza AP. Effects of prematurity on language acquisition and auditory maturation: a systematic review. CoDAS. 2016;28:843-54. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015218
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015218
  • 21.
    Gouveia AS, Oliveira MM, Goulart AL, Azevedo MF, Perissinoto J. Development of speech and hearing skills in prematures adequate and small for gestational age: chronological age between 18 and 36 months. CoDAS. 2020;32:e20180275. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20192018275
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20192018275
  • 22.
    Souza AC, Casais-e-Silva LL, Sena EP. The influence of prematurity on the development of phonological skills. Rev CEFAC. 2019;21:1-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/201921413118
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/201921413118
  • 23.
    Zerbeto AB, Cortelo FM, Filho ÉB. Association between gestational age and birth weight on the language development of Brazilian children: a systematic review. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2015;91:326-32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jped.2014.11.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jped.2014.11.003
  • 24.
    O’Connor D, Green S, Higgins JP, editors. Chapter 5: Defining the review question and developing criteria for including studies. In: Higgins JP, Green S, editors. Cochrane Handbook of Systematic Reviews of Intervention. Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470712184.ch5
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470712184.ch5
  • 25.
    Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
  • 26.
    Effective Public Health Practice Project. Quality assessment tool for quantitative studies. Eff Public Heal Pract Proj. 2010;2-5.
  • 27.
    Thomas BH, Ciliska D, Dobbins M, Micucci S. A process for systematically reviewing the literature: providing the research evidence for public health nursing interventions. Worldviews Evidence-Based Nurs. 2004;1:176-84. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1524-475x.2004.04006.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1524-475x.2004.04006.x
  • 28.
    Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Int J Surg. 2014;12:1495-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2014.07.013
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2014.07.013
  • 29.
    Oliveira LN, Lima MC, Gonçalves VM. Follow-up of low birth weight infants: language acquisition. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 2003;61:802-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/s0004-282x2003000500019
  • 30.
    Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Takahashi K, Nakamura M, Kageyama M, Kubo T, et al. Neurodevelopment in full-term small for gestational age infants: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. Brain Dev. 2016;38:529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.12.013
  • 31.
    Takeuchi A, Yorifuji T, Nakamura K, Tamai K, Mori S, Nakamura M, et al. Catch-up growth and neurobehavioral development among full-term, small-for-gestational-age children: a nationwide Japanese population-based study. J Pediatr. 2018;192:41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.09.002
  • 32.
    O’Neill SM, Hannon G, Khashan AS, Hourihane JO, Kenny LC, Kiely M, et al. Thin-for-gestational age infants are at increased risk of neurodevelopmental delay at 2 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017;102:F197-202. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-310791
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2016-310791
  • 33.
    Simões RV, Cruz-Lemini M, Bargalló N, Gratacós E, Sanz-Cortés M. Brain metabolite differences in one-year-old infants born small at term and association with neurodevelopmental outcome. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2015;213:210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.04.011
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2015.04.011
  • 34.
    Savchev S, Sanz-Cortes M, Cruz-Martinez R, Arranz A, Botet F, Gratacos E, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome of full-term small-for-gestational-age infants with normal placental function. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2013;42:201-6. https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/uog.12391
  • 35.
    Klarić A, Kolundžić Z, Galić S, Bošnjak V. Language development in preschool children born after asymmetrical intrauterine growth retardation. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2012;16:132-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2011.06.003
  • 36.
    Walker SP, Chang SM, Younger N, Grantham-Mcgregor SM. The effect of psychosocial stimulation on cognition and behaviour at 6 years in a cohort of term, low-birthweight Jamaican children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2010;52:e148-54. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03637.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03637.x
  • 37.
    Castro Conde JR, Campo CG, González NL, Millán B, Barrios D, Sosa A, et al. Assessment of neonatal EEG background and neurodevelopment in full-term small for their gestational age infants. Pediatr Res. 2019;88:91-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-019-0693-0
  • 38.
    Wong HS, Santhakumaran S, Cowan FM, Modi N. Developmental assessments in preterm children: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2016;138:e20160251. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-0251
    » https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-0251
  • 39.
    Vohr B. Speech and language outcomes of very preterm infants. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014;19:78-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2013.10.007
  • 40.
    Santos ES, Kieviet JF, Königs M, Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J. Predictive value of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development on development of very preterm/very low birth weight children: a meta-analysis. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89:487-96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.03.008
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.03.008
  • 41.
    Levine TA, Grunau RE, McAuliffe FM, Pinnamaneni R, Foran A, Alderdice FA. Early Childhood Neurodevelopment After Intrauterine Growth Restriction: a Systematic Review. Pediatrics. 2015;135:126-41. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-1143
    » https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-1143
  • 42.
    Miller SL, Huppi PS, Mallard C. The consequences of fetal growth restriction on brain structure and neurodevelopmental outcome. J Physiol. 2016;594:807-23. https://doi.org/10.1113/jp271402
    » https://doi.org/10.1113/jp271402
  • 43.
    Alves TL, Ribeiro HC, Costa ML, Valois SS. Newborn risk factors according to gestational age: a review. Nutrire Rev Soc Bras Aliment Nutr. 2015;40:376-82.
  • 44.
    Figueras F, Gardosi J. Intrauterine growth restriction: new concepts in antenatal surveillance, diagnosis, and management. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2011;204:288-300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2010.08.055
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2010.08.055
  • 45.
    Figueras F, Oros D, Cruz-Martinez R, Padilla N, Hernandez-Andrade E, Botet F, et al. Neurobehavior in term, small-for-gestational age infants with normal placental function. Pediatrics. 2009;124:e934-41. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3346
    » https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3346
  • 46.
    Geva R, Eshel R, Leitner Y, Valevski AF, Harel S. Neuropsychological outcome of children with intrauterine growth restriction: a 9-year prospective study. Pediatrics. 2006;118:91-100. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2343
    » https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2343
  • 47.
    Geva R, Eshel R, Leitner Y, Fattal-Valevski A, Harel S. Memory functions of children born with asymmetric intrauterine growth restriction. Brain Res. 2006;1117:186-94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.004
  • 48.
    Kisilevsky BS, Davies GA. Auditory processing deficits in growth restricted fetuses affect later language development. Med Hypotheses. 2007;68:620-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2006.08.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2006.08.004
  • 49.
    Wile TL, Borowsky R. What does rapid automatized naming measure? A new RAN task compared to naming and lexical decision. Brain Lang. 2004;90:47-62. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00419-x
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00419-x
  • 50.
    Mousinho R, Alves LM, Luiza NA, Salgado-Azoni CA, Celeste LC, Capelini SA, et al. Leitura, escrita e matemática: do desenvolvimento aos transtornos específicos da aprendizagem. Instituto ABCD; 2020.
  • 51.
    Andrade CR, Béfi-lopes DM, Fernandes FD, Wertzner HF. ABFW - Teste de Linguagem Infantil nas áreas de Fonologia, Vocabulário, Fluência e Pragmática. São Paulo: Prófono; 2004.
  • 52.
    Monteiro-Luperi TI, Befi-Lopes DM, Diniz EM, Krebs VL, Carvalho WB. Linguistic performance in 2 years old preterm, considering chronological age and corrected age. CoDAS. 2016;28:118-22. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015075
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162015075
  • 53.
    Brancalioni AR, Zauza A, Karlinski CD, Quitaiski LF, Thomaz MF. Expressive vocabulary performance of students aged from 4 to 5 years attending public and private schools. Audiol Commun Res. 2018;23:1-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2016-1836
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2016-1836
  • 54.
    Bernthal J, Bankson NW, Flipsen PJ. Articulation and phonological disorders: speech sound disorders in children. 8th ed. New York: PEARSON; 2017.
  • 55.
    Silva IB, Lindau TA, Giacheti CM. Preschool preterm infants’ oral language assessment instruments: a literature review. Rev CEFAC. 2017;19:90-8. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201719112416
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201719112416

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    11 May 2022
  • Date of issue
    2022

History

  • Received
    12 Feb 2021
  • Accepted
    23 June 2021
Sociedade de Pediatria de São Paulo R. Maria Figueiredo, 595 - 10o andar, 04002-003 São Paulo - SP - Brasil, Tel./Fax: (11 55) 3284-0308; 3289-9809; 3284-0051 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: rpp@spsp.org.br